ZeePedia

THE SELF (CONTINUE……….):SELF-SCHEMAS, SELF-COMPLEXITY

<< THE SELF (CONTINUE…….):Journal Activity, POSSIBLE HISTORICAL EFFECTS
PERSON PERCEPTION:Impression Formation, Facial Expressions >>
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
Lesson 12
THE SELF (CONTINUE..........)
Aims
Introducing to students that how people know the self.
Objectives
1. Describing how individuals reflect on individual characteristics
Self schemas
Complexity of selves
Possible selves
Self discrepancies
2. Understanding the social context in the understanding of self
Social comparisons
Self distinctiveness
Introduction
People in western culture may have greater difficulty in knowing who they are today them they did in the
past.
That knowing the self has become progressively more difficult. In earlier times, identity was fixed at birth
by the family's recognized place in society. Today, people in many western cultures have so much freedom
and mobility that they have to "discover" who they are. People may no longer know which of many
possible selves is "the real me".
Ways of achieving self-fulfillment have changed along with ways of defining the self. In the Late Medieval
period, for instance, Christians who observed the church's strictures were guaranteed fulfillment in heaven.
Some psychologists believe that people in the modern era, in contrast, lead empty lives that they try to
fill with meaningless consumption of material goods.
Modern people have a vague idea that they would like to achieve a goal called "self-actualization," but
they are not certain what self-actualization is (Baumeister, 1987). In the "self and society" column, we
see that the relationship between self and society has become progressively worse. People in the late
medieval era derived both their identity and their fulfillment from their recognized place in what they
regarded as a "great chain of being" in which all God's creatures had assigned places. By the early
twentieth century, however, people came to believe that society was standing in the way of their
personal fulfillment, so they became hostile and critical. In the late twentieth century, people know that
society does little to help them in their voyage of self-discovery. People have both a personal identity
and a "collective" or social identity. Personal identity is independent of other people; social identity
consists of being identified with groups or categories. A social identity is more than just a cognitive
category. It is also a network of social relationships (Abrams, 1992; Ethier & Deaux, 1994). People in
modern Western cultures may emphasize the personal identity over the social identity.
However, in traditional cultures generally social identity is emphasized over personal identity.
Two frequently used methods for arriving at a personal identity are: reflecting on individual characteristics
and using the social context.
REFLECTING ON INDIVIDUAL CHARACTERISTICS
The first four processes involve primarily private knowledge. They are ways of thinking about a personal
identity that can occur without reference to other people.
Even a hermit who has lived alone in the woods for many years could sit beneath a tree in solitude and
reflect on his or her self-schemas, self-complexity, possible selves, and self-discrepancies.
51
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
SELF-SCHEMAS
A schema is a set of beliefs that provides an organizing framework for understanding a topic, an event, or a
person. A schema for physiques, for example, might help us to tell the difference between being "fat and
being muscular.
Self-schemas, then, are beliefs that provide an organizing framework to help us understand ourselves. Self-
schemas summarize the personality traits, attitudes, values, interests, and other characteristics that we
attribute to our-"selves. They also include actions and interpersonal relationships.
In one study of self-schemas, the investigator asked college students how independent they were and
whether being independent was important to them-- in other words, whether they had a schema for being
independent (Markus, 1977). Some students, "schematics," claimed that they were fiercely and consistently
independent. Other students, "aschematics," claimed to be independent only part of the time and said that
the trait did not matter to them. The aschematics had self-schemas for other characteristics, but not for
being independent.
Other studies have shown that similar results apply to people who use self-schemas about being introverted
versus outgoing, being masculine versus feminine, or being fat versus thin (Catrambone & Markus, 1987;
Crane & Markus, 1982; Fong & Markus, 1982; Markus, Hamill, & Sends, 1987).
SELF-COMPLEXITY
Some people think of themselves along only one or two dimensions. Children may react extremely to
failure because they have relatively simple self-schemas that include only a "good me" and a "bad me".
Anorexics may become so preoccupied with weighing themselves, examining their bodies in the mirror,
and watching what they eat, that they think of themselves along only the one dimension of being fat versus
thin.
It may be dangerous to have too simple a self-schema. A college woman whose self-schema includes the
traits necessary for success in athletics, courses, making female friends, and-maintaining a satisfactory
relationship with a man, for instance, may have a healthier self-schema than another woman whose self-
schema centers on only body weight and grades.
Students who had simple self-schemas had less positive moods after they learned that they failed than after
they learned that they had succeeded. Students who had complex self-schemas were less influenced by
whether they succeeded or failed. Self-schema complexity (having more than one dimension to the self-
schema) is an effective buffer against stress-related illness and depression (Linville, 1987). The graph given
below describes the research more clearly:
POSSIBLE SELVES
Possible selves are projections of future possibilities for the expected, desired; and feared future self.
Students may have a "graduate" possible self. Anorexics may have a "thin" possible self. Visitors to a
doctor's office may have a "seriously ill" possible self. Possible selves are "the selves we could become,
and the selves we are afraid of becoming" (Markus & Nurius, 1986, p. 954). The present or "now" self
overlaps with but is not the same as the many possible future selves.
Possible selves can also affect the way people behave. People who desire a good possible self and fear a
bad possible self, for instance, may be more motivated to take constructive actions than are people who fear
a bad possible self but cannot imagine a good alternative.
In one study of how to change possible selves, the researchers instilled successful and unsuccessful
possible selves in college students (Ruvolo & Markus, 1992). After they imagined successful or
52
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
unsuccessful future selves, all students participated in two tasks. One task required much persistence. The
other required intense concentration. Students who imagined themselves succeeding persisted longer at the
task that required sheer persistence and worked more accurately at the task that required concentration than
did students who imagined themselves failing. The researchers concluded that our possible future selves are
at least as important as are our present self-schemas, because they can motivate and guide us to success.
Present self-schemas differ in complexity. So do future possible selves. The two "tenses" of the self, how-
ever, are not always identical, nor are their Consequences. A person soon to enter prison might have many
present selves arid few future selves. A soon-to-be-divorced person might have just the opposite.
SELF-DISCREPANCIES
Even within present selves, however, people can reflect on their individual characteristics from different
perspectives. The self might be different when seen from different ones of these perspectives (Higgins,
1987). Present selves can be divided into three types: the actual self, the ideal self, and the ought self.
You can view each of these three types of self from two perspectives: the self as seen by the individual and
as seen by important other people like a parent, a best friend, or a spouse.
The ideal and ought selves derive from early interaction between the child and his or her caretaker
(Higgins, 1989). By giving or withholding their affection, the parents convey the message "this is the type
of person we wish you were," from which the children internalize an ideal self. When children actively
violate norms of acceptable behavior, parents react with criticism or physical punishment. Punishment
conveys the message "this is the way you ought to behave if you knew right from wrong," from which die
children internalize an ought self.
Problems arise when the individual perceives self-discrepancies, which are discrepancies between the
actual self and either the ideal self or the ought self, whether from the individual's perspective or from other
people's perspective. Both children and adults become depressed when other people withhold positive
regard or when they are disappointed in themselves.
When their actual self is discrepant from or fails to live up to their own ideal self they feel dejected. When
their actual self is discrepant from an important other person's ideal for them they feel ashamed.
The two major types of self-discrepancy thus elicit different types of emotional reaction. Discrepancies
between the actual self and the ideal self make people feel sad; discrepancies between the actual self and
the ought self make people feel anxious and agitated (Higgins, 1989).
USING THE SOCIAL CONTEXT
Linville's study (1987)
To arrive at a personal identity, then, people
usually find it necessary to use the social context.
7
Two processes (social comparison and self-
distinctiveness) that use other people in the social
6
context to decide "Who am I?"
5
4
Success experience
SOCIAL COMPARISON
3
Failure experience
2
The self concept depends importantly on the
1
attributes of others who are either selected or
0
available  for  comparison  (Pelham,  1991;
Simple self-
Complex self-
schema
schema
Rosenberg, 1993).
Social comparison occurs when people use others as standards of comparison against which to
evaluate their own opinions, attributes, and abilities (Festinger, 1954). For some characteristics, no
objective standard exists (Kruglanski & Mayseless, 1990). Am I thin enough to be attractive and suc-
cessful? Do I have many friends? Such questions are difficult to People arrive at a personal identity by
using the social context. They would not know how they compared to other people answer in absolute
terms.
53
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
Lacking objective standards, people draw inferences by comparing themselves, to others. But which
others? Sometimes we get to choose from many possible others the specific people with whom we want
to compare ourselves. At other times, we have no choice (Wood, 1989). Sometimes we assume our
personal identity and compare the self with other individuals. At other times we assume our social
identity and compare ourselves with members of our own or rival groups (Brewer & Weber, 1994). In
either case, the result of social comparison has an important influence on who we think we are.
People sometimes prefer to learn about people who are similar to them. At other times, they prefer to
learn about people who are a little better or a little worse. Comparing yourself with someone who is a
little better is called upward comparison. Comparing yourself with someone who is a little worse is called
downward comparison. Which makes people feel better about them? It depends. Upward comparison can
lower people's self-esteem by suggesting that they are not performing as well as they might. It can also
raise self-esteem by suggesting that improvement is possible (Testa & Major, 1990). Conversely,
downward comparison can raise people's self-esteem by suggesting that they are doing very well.
The impact of either upward or downward social comparison on a person's self-concept depends on the
reasonfor making the comparison (Kruglanski & Mayseless, 1987, 1988). If the goal is to derive inspiration
that "I can improve," then upward comparison has a positive impact on the self. If people want reassurance
that "things could be worse," then downward comparison is a better strategy for retaining a positive
self-image. People tend to prefer whichever type of comparison protects their self-esteem and gives them
hope for the future
People do not always have a choice, however, about the others who are available for social comparison.
The goal of social comparison and self-reflection, then, is not so much to attain self-knowledge as to make
people feel better about themselves. They are happier when they can selectively remember more positive
than negative life events. If tragedy "makes sense," emotions and stress-related health problems may be
moderated. The ultimate threat to self is death. To avoid debilitating anxiety over their own eventual
demise, people rely on reassuring "cultural worldviews" such as patriotism or religion
SELF DISTINCTIVENESS
Logically, people can only be aware of their own attributes by contrast with other people. Adults would not
think of themselves as adult if people were born mature and were never children. Men would not find it
important to identify themselves as men if there were no women. To a large degree, then, the self-concept
consists of ways in which we are distinctive or different from other people (Johnson & Boyd, 1995).
People of all ages are more likely to include a characteristic as part of their self-schema if they are
surrounded by other people who do not share the characteristic {McGuire & McGuire, 1988). College
students of nontraditional age, for instance, are more likely than traditional age students to mention their
age when they describe themselves (Kite, 1992). The spontaneous self-concept, then, depends importantly
on ways in which we are different from the particular set of other people with whom we happen to have
contact.
Readings
Franzoi, S.L. (2006). Social Psychology. New York: McGraw Hill. Chapter 3.
Lord, C.G. (1997). Social Psychology. Orlando: Harcourt Brace and Company. Chapter 5.
54
Table of Contents:
  1. INTRODUCTION TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY:Readings, Main Elements of Definitions
  2. INTRODUCTION TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY:Social Psychology and Sociology
  3. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY:Scientific Method
  4. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY:Evaluate Ethics
  5. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH PROCESS, DESIGNS AND METHODS (CONTINUED)
  6. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OBSERVATIONAL METHOD
  7. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CORRELATIONAL METHOD:
  8. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY EXPERIMENTAL METHOD
  9. THE SELF:Meta Analysis, THE INTERNET, BRAIN-IMAGING TECHNIQUES
  10. THE SELF (CONTINUED):Development of Self awareness, SELF REGULATION
  11. THE SELF (CONTINUE…….):Journal Activity, POSSIBLE HISTORICAL EFFECTS
  12. THE SELF (CONTINUE……….):SELF-SCHEMAS, SELF-COMPLEXITY
  13. PERSON PERCEPTION:Impression Formation, Facial Expressions
  14. PERSON PERCEPTION (CONTINUE…..):GENDER SOCIALIZATION, Integrating Impressions
  15. PERSON PERCEPTION: WHEN PERSON PERCEPTION IS MOST CHALLENGING
  16. ATTRIBUTION:The locus of causality, Stability & Controllability
  17. ATTRIBUTION ERRORS:Biases in Attribution, Cultural differences
  18. SOCIAL COGNITION:We are categorizing creatures, Developing Schemas
  19. SOCIAL COGNITION (CONTINUE…….):Counterfactual Thinking, Confirmation bias
  20. ATTITUDES:Affective component, Behavioral component, Cognitive component
  21. ATTITUDE FORMATION:Classical conditioning, Subliminal conditioning
  22. ATTITUDE AND BEHAVIOR:Theory of planned behavior, Attitude strength
  23. ATTITUDE CHANGE:Factors affecting dissonance, Likeability
  24. ATTITUDE CHANGE (CONTINUE……….):Attitudinal Inoculation, Audience Variables
  25. PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION:Activity on Cognitive Dissonance, Categorization
  26. PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION (CONTINUE……….):Religion, Stereotype threat
  27. REDUCING PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION:The contact hypothesis
  28. INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION:Reasons for affiliation, Theory of Social exchange
  29. INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION (CONTINUE……..):Physical attractiveness
  30. INTIMATE RELATIONSHIPS:Applied Social Psychology Lab
  31. SOCIAL INFLUENCE:Attachment styles & Friendship, SOCIAL INTERACTIONS
  32. SOCIAL INFLUENCE (CONTINE………):Normative influence, Informational influence
  33. SOCIAL INFLUENCE (CONTINUE……):Crimes of Obedience, Predictions
  34. AGGRESSION:Identifying Aggression, Instrumental aggression
  35. AGGRESSION (CONTINUE……):The Cognitive-Neo-associationist Model
  36. REDUCING AGGRESSION:Punishment, Incompatible response strategy
  37. PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR:Types of Helping, Reciprocal helping, Norm of responsibility
  38. PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR (CONTINUE………):Bystander Intervention, Diffusion of responsibility
  39. GROUP BEHAVIOR:Applied Social Psychology Lab, Basic Features of Groups
  40. GROUP BEHAVIOR (CONTINUE…………):Social Loafing, Deindividuation
  41. up Decision GROUP BEHAVIOR (CONTINUE……….):GroProcess, Group Polarization
  42. INTERPERSONAL POWER: LEADERSHIP, The Situational Perspective, Information power
  43. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY APPLIED: SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY IN COURT
  44. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY APPLIED: SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY IN CLINIC
  45. FINAL REVIEW:Social Psychology and related fields, History, Social cognition