|
|||||
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
Lesson
06
CAUSAL
ATTRIBUTIONS IN COMPETITIVE
SITUATIONS
A competitive
situation is defined as one in which
participants expect that their
performance will be
evaluated
by
others in some way. It is an
opportunity to compete with
others for some internal or
external reward.
Regarding
the internal and external dimensions of
attributions, several lines of
research have evolved. In this
lecture
we will be looking at two
concepts: Locus of causality,
and the covariation
principle.
Locus
of Causality
Previously
called locus of control,
locus of causality is the extent to which
people believe they are
responsible
for
their behavioral outcomes. To avoid
confusion between the terms locus of
control and controllability,
locus
of
control dimension is renamed as the "locus of
causality". Locus of causality
can be of two types;
internal
locus
of causality, and external locus of
causality. People with
internal locus of causality
tend to believe their
behaviors
influence outcomes. They believe that
they are responsible for the
outcome and view themselves
as in
control
of their destiny. While those
with an external locus of causality,
tend to attribute outcomes to
outside
forces
such as fate, chance, and
other people. They believe
that their outcome is
dependent on external sources
and
so they see themselves at the mercy of
the external events.
Perhaps
the person most responsible
for developing the conceptual framework
for the locus of causality
was
Julian
B. Rotter. Rotter (1966) developed the
29-items Internal-External Locus of
Control Scale to measure
the
extent
to which people believe they possess
some control over their
lives. Rotter (1971) stated
the following
generalities
about locus of causality: (1)
children coming from a lower
socioeconomic environment tend to
be
external,
(2) children tend to become
more internal with age,
and (3) highly external people feel they
are at the
mercy
of their environment and are
continually being manipulated by outside
forces.
While
it is not appropriate for human
beings to always give internal
attributions to outcomes, research
makes it
clear
that in the sports environment it is
better to have an internal as opposed to
external locus of causality.
Research
suggests that an internal
orientation is more mature
than an external orientation.
A
study noted that gymnastic
coaches typically rank-order
their gymnasts from poorest to
best for gymnastic
meets,
on the theory that judges
expect scores to improve as the
meet progresses. The results
showed that
judges
who themselves had an
internal locus of control
were uninfluenced by order.
Conversely, judges who
had
external
locus of control were influenced by
order. This suggests that the
judges who had an internal
orientation
were
able to overcome the order
bias and judge solely on the
basis of performance.
Even
though an internal locus of
causality appears to be preferable to an external
locus of causality, this should
not
be construed to mean that
all external attributions are
immature. Sometimes external attributions
are
appropriate
and expected. For example,
it would be completely normal for
athletes to blame defeat on
poor
officiating
if their team had been
called for twice as many
fouls as the other
team.
Covariation
Principle
A
person's attributions for
success or failure can be predicted on
the basis of the performance of
others
on
the same task. This phenomenon has
been named the covariation
principle. According to this
principle,
when the performance of others agrees
with the performance of the participants,
attributions
will
be external. If the performance of others
disagrees with the performance of
participant, attributions
will
be internal. For example, if I
beat someone in tennis whom
everyone else has lost to, I
will certainly
attribute
my victory to an internal cause
such as superior ability. Conversely, if
I defeat someone in
tennis
everyone else has defeated,
it is very likely that I will
attribute my success to an external
cause
such
as my opponents' low ability. When
your performance agrees with
the performance of others,
your
attributions are likely to be external in
nature (e.g., task
difficulty or luck). Conversely, when
your
19
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
performance
disagrees with the performance of
others, your attributions
are likely to be internal
in
nature
(e.g., ability or
effort).
Perceived
Causality and Emotional
Response
Emotional
response to athletic outcomes occurs on
two different levels;
attribution free, and
attribution
dependent.
At the initial or primitive level,
emotional responses are said
to be attribution free. This
initial
emotional response is given before the
athlete had a chance to
consider the cause of the
outcome.
The
initial response of an athlete to
success is generally happiness of
joy. The initial response of
an
athlete
to failure is generally disappointment or
sadness.
At
the second level, emotional response
arise in direct response to causal
attributions as to why the
outcome
occurred. These emotions are
attribution dependent, and
are said to be distinct and
separate
from
the initial attribution-free emotional
response. Theses attribution-dependent
emotions reveal a
great
deal about how the athlete
feels about why the outcome
occurred.
The
affect-attribution relationship provides the sport
psychologist with an excellent
tool for
understanding
the cognitions and thoughts of the athlete.
Attributions are primarily
linked to the
emotions
of anger, gratitude, guilt, pity,
shame pride, self confidence and
competence. Failure that is
linked
to personal controllability results in
the emotions of shame, guilt, and
perhaps depression.
Conversely,
success that is linked to
personal controllability results in the
emotions of pride, self
confidence
and competence.
Stability
dimensions are linked to
feelings of hopefulness or hopelessness.
If you lose and attribute
the
cause
to something that is not going to
change (stable cause) then
hopelessness, or the feeling that
failure
will continue , is the expected affective
response. Conversely, if you
win and attribute the
cause
to
a stable attribution, the expected
emotional response is hopefulness, or the
expectation that you
will
continue
to win. On the other hand, if an
athlete loses but attributes the
cause to something that
is
unstable
and can change, for
example, not enough effort,
then he can be hopeful that
things will be
different
next time with a little more
effort. But if an athlete
wins but attributes the cause to
something
unstable,
such as good luck, then the
expected affect is uncertainty.
The
kinds of attributions that young athletes
make in response to success
and failure are closely
linked
to
their feelings of self-esteem
and self-confidence and suffering.
Individuals suffering from low
esteem
are
more likely than individuals
high in self-esteem to internalize a
failure and respond with
negative
affect.
Expectancy
and Attribution
A
hopeful individual has
expectations that future
contests will result in
success, whereas a
hopeless
individual
has expectations that future
contests will result in another
defeat or failure. This has
been
proven
in the medical field too, as a
hopeful patient suffering from a
mental/physical disease has better
chance
of recovering faster or surviving longer
than a hopeless patient. Given
that attributions are
linked
to emotions and that emotions are
linked to cognitions, we can see the critical
importance of
attributions
for athletic outcomes.
Learned
Helplessness
Learned
helplessness is a situation where an
individual thinks that he
has no control over the
situation
and
just gives up. This concept
was developed by Seligman (1995)
and he defines learned
helplessness
in
terms of the phrase "giving up
without even trying". It is
caused by the perception that one
has no control
over
his failures, and that
failure is inevitable.
20
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
Research
done on children shows that
"learned helpless" children
who show a deterioration of
performance
under
the threat of failure tend to attribute
their failure to stable
factors such as lack of
ability. Conversely,
"learned
helpless" children who show
enhanced performance under the threat of
failure tend to choose
unstable
factors
such as luck or lack of
effort. Attributing success to
stable factors suggests to the
child that success is
a
realistic
expectation for the future. On the other
hand, attributing failure to a
stable factor suggests to the
child
that
failure is a realistic expectation for
the future.
Attributional
Training
Research
with attributional training
has clearly shown that
planned interventions can alter ways
that an athlete
perceives
outcome, as well as alter actual
performance. A functional attribution
strategy is one in which
athletes
are
taught to explain the causes of a failure
outcome as being controllable and
unstable. A dysfunctional
attribution
strategy is one in which
athletes are taught to explain the cause
of a failure outcome as being
uncontrollable
and stable. Research with
children suggests that
attributional training can
positively influence a
child's
future expectations and
performance.
Attributional
training can also be effective
with adults, although not as
effective as it is with children.
Adults
respond
well to attributional training as
long as their perceived
competence is not too low.
Athletes with
maladaptive
attributional patterns give failure
attributions that are more
internal, stable, uncontrollable,
and
global
than those of the nonmaladaptive
athletes.
To
help athletes choose
suitable attributions, the following
steps are
recommended:
1.
Record
and classify attributions
that athletes make to
successful and unsuccessful
outcomes
2.
For
each outcome, discuss with
the athlete causes or attributions
that might lead to a
greater
expectancy
for success and increased
effort.
3.
Provide
an attributional training program for
athletes who consistently give
attributions that lead
to
negative
implications for the future
outcomes.
4.
For
best results, combine planned goal
setting with attributional
training.
References
Cox,
H. Richard. (2002). Sport Psychology:
Concepts and Applications.
(Fifth Edition). New York:
McGraw-
Hill
Companies
Lavallec.
D., Kremer, J., Moran,
A., & Williams. M. (2004)
Sports Psychology: Contemporary Themes.
New
York:
Palgrave Macmillan
Publishers
21
Table of Contents:
|
|||||