|
|||||
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
Lesson
05
CAUSAL
ATTRIBUTION IN SPORT
Attribution
Theory
The
key element in attribution
theory is perception.
When athletes are asked," to
what do you attribute
your
great success?" they are being
asked for their perceptions.
The fact that their
perceptions of why
they
are successful may be completely
erroneous is beside the point.
The manner in which
athletes
answer
questions like these reveals
their perceptual
beliefs.
Attribution
theory is a cognitive approach to
motivation. It assumes that
people, understand,
and
predict
events based upon their
cognitive perception. According to
attribution theory, the intent
of
every
human being is to explain his
own actions in terms of
their perceived causes.
Fritz Heider (1944,
1958)
described his theory as one
of common sense, or "naive
psychology".
This
is a complex theory in which perceived
attributions are viewed as greatly
influencing a person's
actions,
feelings, confidence, and
motivation. How and athlete
feels about herself is directly related
to
the
athlete's perception of cause and effect.
The attributions that
athletes select reveal their
motivational
structures.
Furthermore, helping athletes to change
their perceptions can have a
significant effect on
their
motivation to achieve. For this
reason, motivation and
attribution theory are very
closely related.
For
example, some young people feel they fail
because they lack natural ability.
Since natural ability is
relatively
permanent, it is hard for those
children to see that things will
ever change for the
better.
However,
if the young athletes can be encouraged
to consider bad luck or lack
of effort as a cause
for
their
failure, they need not feel that things
cannot change always try
harder.
The
Attributional Model
The
basic attribution model was
proposed by Heider (1944,
1958). However, several
significant
contributions
by Weiner (1972, 1979, and
1985) have made it much
more useful. Most
recently,
contributions
by Russell (1982) and by
McAuley, Duncan, and Russell
(1992) have improved our
ability
to
measure attribution.
Fritz
Heider's Contribution
The
basis of Heider's model was the notion
that people strive for prediction
and understanding of
daily
events
in order to give their lives
stability and predictability.
Outcomes are attributed
internally to the
person
(personal force) or externally to the environment
(environmental force). Effective
personal
force
is composed of the attributional factors
ability
and
effort,
while effective environmental force
is
composed
of the attributional factors task
difficulty and
luck.
According
to Heider, an interaction occurs
between the personal force of ability
and the environmental
force
of task difficulty that
yields a separate dimension referred to as
can (or cannot). If a task
is
difficult
and yet is accomplished, it
must be due to great
ability. However, depending on the
difficulty
of
the task and the ability of the
subject, several other
attributions can give rise to the
can or cannot
dimension.
The
highly unstable factor of
luck also enters into
many attribution situations.
Luck is an environmental
factor
that can favorably or
unfavorably change an outcome in an
unsystematic way. All these
factors
(trying,
ability, task difficulty,
and luck) combine to result in a behavioral
outcome, to which an
16
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
individual
attributes a cause. Heider reasoned
that the personal and the
environmental components of
causation
are
additive. Thus the following formula
represents his
reasoning:
Behavioral
outcome = personal force + environmental
force
Bernard
Weiner's Contributions
Using
Heider's basic formulation, Weiner
(1972) made several significant
contributions to the attribution
model
that
made it easier to understand
and apply in achievement situations.
Weiner took Heider's four
main factors
and
restructured them into two
main causal dimensions.
Theses two dimensions he
labeled stability
and
locus
of
control.
Weiner
then incorporated Heider's four main
factors into his two-dimensional
classification scheme for
causal
attribution.
Ability was classified as being
internal and stable, effort
as internal and unstable,
task difficulty as
external
and stable, luck as external
and unstable. In his later
writing's Weiner (1979,
1985) clarified that a
third
dimension
named controllability
must
be included in the attribution model.
The
inclusion of this third dimension created a few
conceptual problems that had
to be addressed. The
first
problem
was how to differentiate
between the dimension of locus of control
and the new dimension of
controllability.
He solved this problem by renaming the
locus of control dimension locus
and clarifying the
distinction
between the two dimensions. He explained
that locus of causality has
to do with whether an
outcome
was perceived by the individual to be
controllable or uncontrollable.
Dan
Russell's And Ed Mcauleys's
Contributions
To
deal with attribution
distortion and misclassification,
Russell (1982) developed the Casual
Dimension Scale
(CDS). In
using the CDS, athletes are
asked to indicate their perceived
cause for an outcome, and
then to rate
the
cause relative to nine questions.
The scale is composed of
three questions for each of
the dimensions of
locus,
stability, and controllability.
Since its development in 1982, the
Casual Dimension Scale has
been used
extensively
by researchers.
An
article by McAuley, Duncan, and
Russell (19920) documents the development
of the Casual
Dimension
Scale2
(CDS2), a revision of the
original version of the scale. The
revised version differs from the
original in
that
it comprises four rather than
three casual dimension scales.
The four dimensions of the
CDS2 are locus of
control,
stability, personal control,
and external control. The
original CDS scale failed to
distinguish between
causes
that were controlled by the
individual and those
controlled by other people.
Attributions were simply
controllable
of uncontrollable, with no clear
indication of who was
controlling the cause.CDS2 measures
four
specific
dimensions of causality.
Other
Considerations
A
number of conceptual problems persist.
For instance, researchers
and practitioners may fail to
recognize that
the
kinds of attributions people make are
based on a socialization process
that may vary across
cultures.
Socialization
plays an important part in the
emphasis that we place on
attributions. Attributions depend on
what
we
learn to value. Differences in
Socializations will undoubtedly affect
the kinds of attributions made.
In
addition to social-cultural differences,
we also have evidence that
young track and field
athletes' attributions
do
not differ as a function of
gender, but do differ as a
function of race/ethnicity.
A
second problem that has
often plagued attribution
research is that the experimenter can
bias a subject's
perception
of outcome. In many sports
related attribution studies,
subjects do not perceive
themselves to be
succeeding
or failing until the researcher
biases their perceptions b
asking, "to what do you
attribute your
success(or
failure). Sometimes success and
failure are perceived
differently by researchers and
athlete. For
17
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
example,
let's say I play tennis with
one of the world's best
players. I don't expect to
win but if I can
win
one or two games, I will
consider myself a
success.
References
Cox,
H. Richard. (2002). Sport Psychology:
Concepts and Applications.
(Fifth Edition). New
York:
McGraw-Hill
Companies
Lavallec.
D., Kremer, J., Moran,
A., & Williams. M. (2004)
Sports Psychology: Contemporary
Themes.
New
York: Palgrave Macmillan
Publishers
18
Table of Contents:
|
|||||