|
|||||
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
LESSON
12
ATTENTION
AND CONCENTRATION IN SPORT
According
to William James (1890),
attention is the taking possession by the
mind, in clear and vivid
form, of
one
out of what seem several
simultaneously possible objects or
trains of thought. It Implies withdrawal
from
some
things in order to deal effectively
with others. In sport, nothing
can be more important than
paying
attention
to the object at hand. Sport psychologists
have recognized the importance of
attention, and that it
can
be
very complex. This topic
consists of ten important concepts;
first five will be discussed
in this lecture, and
the
remaining in lecture thirteen.
Important
concepts include the following:
1.
Information
processing
2.
Memory
systems
3.
Measuring
information
4.
Selective
attention
5.
Information
processing capacity
6.
Attentional
narrowing
7.
Being
in the zone
8.
Measuring
attentional focus
9.
Attention
control training
10.
Associative
versus dissociative attentional
styles.
Information
Processing
There
are two basic approaches to
explaining behavior. The
first and probably better understood is
the
behavioral,
or stimulus-response
approach. In this
way of looking at things, the world is
explained through a
series
of stimulus-response (S-R) connections.
With animals this approach
has been extremely
successful, but
for
human beings it seems too
simplistic. There seems to be more to
human behavior than the
simple act of
strengthening
the bond between a stimulus
and a response. Certainly, a
great deal goes on in the
brain between
the
time that a stimulus is given and the
time that a response in initiated.
This notion is accepted by
the
cognitive
psychologists and is referred to as the
information
processing model of
behavior. The
information
processing
model contains a stimulus and a
response, but a large number of
mental operations occur
between
the
two. For a person to
experience a stimulus and
respond to at a later time, there
must be a memory
storage
capacity.
That is, the person must
have a memory, or place to save
important information. Once
the
information
has been saved, the person
must be able to reactivate or retrieve
it. Retrieval enables us to use
the
information
to make decisions about forthcoming
responses. This is information processing
in action, and it
takes
place constantly on the athletic
field.
Memory
Systems
There
are three basic memory
systems:
Sensory
information store
Short-Term
Memory (STM)
Long-Term
Memory (LTM)
Sensory
Information Store
The
first stage in the human
memory system, sometimes
called sensory register.
This storage system is
capable
of
holding large amounts of
sensory information for a
brief amount of time before most of it is
lost.
Information
is thought to remain in the sensory
register for up to one-half
second before it is either lost or
transferred
to a more permanent storage
system.
39
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
Short-Term
Memory (STM)
The
short term memory (STM) is the center, or
crossroads, of activity in the
information processing
system.
Information comes into STM
for rehearsal from both
sensory store and permanent
memory.
Information
that comes into STM
from the sensory store is
often new or original
information. If we do
not
rehearse and memorize it
quickly, we will likely
forget it. For example, when
one gets a new
telephone
number, he tends to repeat it several
times to remember it. The
quality of rehearsal
determines
whether or not information in STM is
passed on to long-term memory. The
absolute
capacity
of short-term memory is relatively limited,
for example, it would be
difficult for an
average
person
to retain more than seven
separate words or numbers in
STM at one time.
Long-Term
Memory (LTM)
Whereas
information in short-term memory is
present for only a brief
period of time, information in
long-term
memory (LTM) is relatively permanent.
Once information is stored in
LTM, it is theoretically
permanent.
Information in LTM can be continually
upgraded, reorganized, and
strengthened. New
information
can also be added to
LTM.
Measuring
Information
Psychologists
can measure the amount of information
that is conveyed by a particular problem
or task.
The
amount of information conveyed or transmitted by a
particular problem is measured in bits
of
information,
(short for "binary digit").
The more bits of information
conveyed the more difficult
the
problem
being presented.
There
is a clear relationship between an
athlete's skill and
information conveyed. As skill
increases,
information
conveyed increase. The more
information conveyed by an offensive player, the
more
difficult
it is going to be for the defensive
player to respond. Applying this
principle in the game of
cricket,
a highly skilled bowler will be conveying
more information and making it
difficult for the
batsman
to respond.
Selective
Attention
Selective
attention is the ability to gate
out, or ignore, irrelevant sensory
information, and to
pay
attention
to relevant information. Each of us has
experienced the feeling of over
stimulation that can
result
in an inability to concentrate. If it
were not for our
ability to concentrate on one or
two relevant
items
at a time, we simply could not function.
The ability to selectively attend to
appropriate stimuli is
critical
in most athletic situations. In
basketball, an athlete must
concentrate on the basket
while
shooting
a free throw rather than being
distracted by the noise from the crowd.
In cricket, an athlete
facing
the bowler must concentrate on
ball instead of being distracted by
thoughts of a previous play.
Some
athletes are better than others at
selectively attending to important cues.
This is one difference
between
the good athlete and the outstanding
athlete.
For
highly trained and skilled athletes, the
process of selective attention is very
efficient. When skilled
basketball
players step up to the free
throw line, they refuse to
allow anyone or anything
besides the
task
at hand to capture their attention.
Coaches refer to this process as
"concentration". However,
some
athletes never so learn how
to cope with distraction. Every
little thing distracts them,
or they
concentrate
on wrong things.
Limited
Information Processing
Capacity
An
alternative approach to studying attention is to
view it in terms of information
processing capacity,
or
space. We have the capacity to attend to
more than one thing at a
time. For example, a
person
driving
40
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
a
car can listen to music,
steer the car, and shift
gears all at the same time. If a
specific task requires all
of the
information
processing space, than that
specific task is selectively
attended to at the expense of all
others. If a
specific
task does not require all of
the available space, then
more than one task
can be attended to at one
time,
depending
upon the attentional demands of the
second task. this is called the
capacity model of selective
attention.
In
the capacity model of attention, more
than one piece of input
can be attended to at one time
and more than
one
response can be made at one
time, if the demands on available space
are not too
severe.
Wrisberg
and Shea (1978) demonstrated
through the use of the reaction time
probe that the
attentional
demands
of a motor act decrease as learning
increases. In other words as a
motor act becomes automatic
or
learned,
the demands on the limited information
processing capacity of the athlete
decrease, and the athlete
can
attend
to other cues. Reaction time
probe is a technique used in
attention research to determine if a
certain
primary
task requires information
processing space.
An
important factor that should be
considered is the notion of individual
differences (Keele & Hawkins,
1982).
No
two individuals are alike in
terms of the amount of attention required to
deal with more than
one task at a
time.
Therefore, one should not assume
that two athletes possessing
an equal amount of playing experience
will
perform
the same when confronted
with a multiple-task problem. One athlete
might experience some
difficulty
in
performing tasks A and B together,
but perform task B and C
flawlessly. On the other hand, a
second athlete
might
experience difficulty performing
tasks B and C together, but
experience no difficulty with
tasks A and B
together.
References
Cox,
H. Richard. (2002). Sport Psychology:
Concepts and Applications.
(Fifth Edition). New York:
McGraw-
Hill
Companies
Lavallec.
D., Kremer, J., Moran,
A., & Williams. M. (2004)
Sports Psychology: Contemporary Themes.
New
York:
Palgrave Macmillan
Publishers
41
Table of Contents:
|
|||||