ZeePedia

PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR:Types of Helping, Reciprocal helping, Norm of responsibility

<< REDUCING AGGRESSION:Punishment, Incompatible response strategy
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR (CONTINUE………):Bystander Intervention, Diffusion of responsibility >>
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
Lesson 37
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR
Aims
To introduce psychological aspects of prosocial behaviour
Objectives
Describe different types of helping behavior
·
Discuss different explanations of helping behavior: Why do we help?
·
Evaluate the Bystander Intervention Model
Prosocial Behavior: Chapter Summary
This chapter discusses the basics of helping behavior. Altruism is distinguished from prosocial behavior.
Several theoretical perspectives on helping are considered. These include the evolutionary perspective; the
socio-cultural perspective (focusing on social norms of responsibility, reciprocity, social justice); the
learning perspective (modeling and reinforcement); Latané and Darley's decision-making perspective
(perceiving a need, taking personal responsibility, weighing the costs and benefits, deciding to help and
taking action); and attribution theory's perspective (focusing on our willingness to help those who deserve
help because we attribute their problems to causes out of their control). Factors that influence a potential
helper's likelihood of actually helping are considered, including mood, empathy and personal distress,
personality characteristics, and gender. More specific situational factors that influence the decision to help
are also discussed, including the bystander effect (and explanations for it), environmental conditions, and
time pressures.
Definitions
All of us have experience of helping and being helped by others. Sometime our prosocial behavior involves
little cost, while on other times it involves money, effort, or time. Two kinds of helping behavior exist with
different motives. Nineteenth century philosopher Auguste Comte maintained that egoistic help is based on
egoism; in which the person wants something in return. On the other hand altruistic help is for another
person's welfare. Prosocial, egoistic and altruistic behaviors are distinguished below from each other:
Prosocial Behavior: Voluntary behavior that is carried out to benefit another person
Egoistic helping: A form of helping in which the ultimate goal of the helper is to increase one's own
welfare
Altruistic helping means helping someone when there is no expectation of a reward
Types of Helping (McGuire, 1994)
·  Casual help, e.g., giving directions
·  Substantial help, e.g., lending money
·  Emotional help, e.g., listening
·  Emergency help, e.g., saving someone, helping in crisis
Explanations of helping behavior: Why do we help?
·  An Evolutionary perspective
·  A Sociocultural Perspective: Norms of reciprocity, social responsibility, social justice
·  A Learning Perspective
152
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
Helping is Consistent with Evolutionary Theory
Sometime we help for some personal gain, while on other times we help without any personal motive. Not
only human beings, but many examples of prosocial behavior have been observed among animal species,
e.g., dolphins, lions, chimpanzees, etc. One principle of evolutionary theory is that any social behavior that
enhances reproductive success (the conception, birth, and survival of offspring) will continue to be passed
on from one generation to the next. However, to reproduce, an animal must first survive. Taken together,
there may be mechanisms for the genetic transmission of helpful inclinations from generation to generation.
Evolutionary theorists contend that it is not only personal survival that is important. Rather it is a gene
survival that promotes reproductive fitness.
"Kin selection" provides an explanation for gene survival:
·  There is a preference for helping blood relatives because this will increase the chances for the
helper's genes to pass on to successive generations. Because your blood relatives share many of
your same genes, by promoting their survival you can also preserve your genes even if you don't
survive the helpful act. This principle of kin selection states that you will exhibit preferences for
helping blood relatives because this will increase the odds that your genes will be transmitted to
subsequent generations.
·  Animals help others more who are genetically related.
But People also help non-relatives. How this becomes possible? This is explained by Trivers (1983) in the
next perspective on prosocial behavior explanations.
A Socio-cultural Perspective: Social Norms
Reciprocal helping:
According to this principle, people are likely to help strangers if it is understood that the recipient is
expected to return the favor at some time in future. Trivers (1983) believes that reciprocal helping is most
likely to evolve in a species when certain conditions exist. Three of these conditions are:
·  Social group living, so that individuals have ample opportunity to give and receive help.
·  Mutual dependence, in which species survival depends on cooperation, and
·  The lack of rigid dominance hierarchies, so that reciprocal helping will enhance each animal's
power.
Considerable research supports both kin selection and reciprocal helping among humans and other animals.
For example, when threatened by predators, squirrels are much more likely to warn genetically related
squirrels with which they live than unrelated squirrels or those from other areas. Similarly, across a wide
variety of human cultures, relatives receive more help than non-relatives, especially if the help involves
considerable costs, such as being a kidney donor (Borgida et al., 1992). Reciprocal helping is also common
in humans, and, consistent with evolutionary-based mechanisms to prevent cheating, when people are
unable to reciprocate, they tend to experience guilt and
Figure  1  illustrates  the  power  of
reciprocity:
shame.
Three social norms that serve as guidelines for
prosocial
behavior
deal
with
reciprocity,
responsibility, and justice. The first of these prosocial
norms, the norm of reciprocity, is based on
maintaining fairness in social relationships. This norm
prescribes that people should be paid back for
whatever they give us. This norm also explains the
discomfort that people typically experience when they
receive help but cannot give something back in return.
Norm of responsibility:
In comparison to the reciprocity norm, the other two
153
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
prosocial norms dictate that people should help due to a greater awareness of what is right. For instance, the
norm of social responsibility states that we should help when others are in need and dependent on us.
Acting on this norm, adults feel responsible for the health and safety of children, teachers have a sense of
duty and obligation to their students, and police and fire-fighters believe they must help even at the risk of
their own lives. This social responsibility norm requires help-givers to render assistance regardless of the
recipient's worthiness and without an expectation of being rewarded.
Norm of social justice:
In contrast to the dependent-driven social responsibility norm, the norm of social justice stipulates that
people should help only when they believe that others deserve assistance. People become entitled to the
deserving label by either possessing socially desirable personality characteristics or by engaging in socially
desirable behaviors. Thus, according to the social justice norm, if "good" people encounter unfortunate
circumstances, they deserve our help and we have a duty to render assistance.
A Learning Perspective
Observational learning in children:
According to social learning theorists, observational learning or modeling can influence the development of
helping in at least two ways (Rosenkoetter, 1999; Rushton, 1980). First, it can initially teach children how
to engage in helpful actions. Second, it can show children what is likely to happen when they actually
engage in helpful (or selfish) behavior. In this learning process, what models say and what they do both
shape the observers' prosocial behaviors. For example, in one study, sixth-grade girls played a game to win
chips that could be traded for candy and toys (Midlarsky et al,, 1973). Prior to actually playing, each of the
girls watched a woman play the game. In the charitable condition, the adult put some of the chips she won
into a jar labeled "money for poor children" and then urged the girl to think about the poor children who
would "love to receive the prizes these chips can buy." In the selfish condition, the adult model also urged
the child to donate chips to the poor children, but she did so after putting all her chips into a jar labeled "my
money." Results indicated a clear effect of prosocial modeling. Girls who had observed the charitable
model donated more chips to the poor than those who had seen the selfish model.
Prosocial modeling in adults:
Modeling prosocial behavior is not confined to children. In one study conducted in a natural setting,
motorists who simply saw someone helping a woman change a flat tire were more likely to later stop and
assist a second woman who was in a similar predicament (Bryan & Test, 1967). In another experiment
(Rushton & Campbell, 1977), female college students interacted with a friendly woman as part of a study
on social interaction (this was not the true purpose, and the woman was a confederate of the researchers).
When the fabricated study was completed, the two women left the lab together and passed a table staffed by
people asking for blood donations. When participants were asked first, only 25% agreed, and none actually
followed through on their pledge six weeks later. However, when the confederate was asked first and
signed up to donate blood, 67% of the participants also agreed to give blood, and 33% actually fulfilled
their commitment.
Modelling helping behaviour
Figure 2 more clearly illustrates the findings of this
study:
70
Rewarding prosocial behavior:
60
Although observing the prosocial actions of others can
50
shape children's and adults' own helping, the
40
consequences of their actions will often determine
30
whether they continue to engage in prosocial behavior.
20
Social rewards, such as praise, are generally more
effective reinforcers than material rewards, such as
10
money (Grusec, 1991). In one such experiment
0
Participant asked
Confederate
conducted by Rushton and Goody Teachman (1978),
first
asked first
154
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
children were first induced to behave generously by having generosity modeled to them as in the previously
described game-token studies. When the children donated some of their winnings to an orphan named
Bobby, the model either praised the child for his or her imitative generosity (reward condition) by saying
"Good for you, that's really nice of you," or scolded the child (punishment condition) by saying "That's
kind of silly for you to give to Bobby. Now you will have fewer tokens for yourself." There was also a no-
reinforcement condition in which the adult said nothing. As you can see in figure 3, children who were
praised gave more to Bobby on later trials than did children who were scolded. The effects of being either
rewarded or punished for prosocial behavior were so strong that they still influenced how much the
children gave to Bobby two weeks later. The results are demonstrated in Figure 3 as given below:
Another study demonstrates that verbal praise or
scolding by an adult model can either strengthen or
weaken children's level of generosity (Moss & Page,
1972). In the reward condition, the woman asking for
directions rewarded her helper by saying, "Thank you
very much, I really appreciate this." In contrast, in the
punishment condition the woman responded to help by
saying, "I can't understand what you're saying, never
mind, I'll ask someone else." Researchers found that
when people were rewarded by the first woman, 90%
of them helped the second woman. However, when
punished by the first woman, only 40% helped in the
later situation. As in the study with children, this adult
study suggests that people's future decisions to help
are often influenced by the degree to which current helpful efforts are met by praise or rebuke.
When do we help?
On March 13, 1964, Kitty Genovese was stabbed on
her way back to home. A man stabbed her with knife
near her apartment building in New York at 3.20 a.m.
Her cries rang out in the night but nobody came for
help while at least 38 of her neighbors were watching
from the windows. The apathy of her neighbors was
the topic of news stories, and people's dinner
conversations. Two people who discussed the murder
at length were social psychologists John Darley and
Bibb Latane.
Bystander intervention model eventually emerged as a
result of these dinner discussions.
Bystander  intervention  involves  a  series  of
decisions
Although people often see people in need of help, they
sometimes don't go and offer it themselves. People decide whether or not to offer assistance based on a
variety of perceptions and evaluations. Help is offered only if a person answers "yes" at each step. The
bystander intervention model maintains that there are four stages which must be gone through before
helping occurs.
As it can be seen in Figure 4 given below that at each point in this five-step process, one decision results in
no help being given, while the other decision takes the bystander one step closer to intervention.
155
img
Social Psychology (PSY403)
VU
The first thing that you, as a potential helper, must do is notice that something unusual is happening.
Unfortunately, in many social settings, countless sights and sounds flood our senses. Because it is
impossible to attend to all this stimuli, and because we may be preoccupied with something else, a cry for
help could conceivably go completely unnoticed. This stimulus overload effect is more likely to occur in
densely populated urban environments than in rural settings (Milgram, 1970). Indeed, it is one of the likely
reasons why there is a negative correlation between population density and helping (Levine, 2003).
Another reason is that sometimes it is difficult to notice things out of the ordinary as what is unusual in one
setting may be a normal occurrence in another. As a bystander to an emergency, if you do indeed notice
that something unusual is happening you move to the second step in the decision-making process: deciding
whether something is wrong and help is needed. Returning to the previous example, if you pass by an
unconscious man on the sidewalk you may ask yourself, "Did he suffer a heart attack or is he merely
sleeping. When you define the situation as an emergency, the bystander intervention model states that the
third decision you must make is determining the extent to which you have responsibility to help. According
to Latane and Darley, one factor that may play a role in your decision to help or not is whether an
appropriate authority figure is nearby. Let's continue this hypothetical emergency situation, but now
imagine that there is no police car in sight. Faced with the reality of a clear emergency, you still may not
help if you convince yourself that all the other motorists watching this incident could help just as well as
you. The presence of these other potential helpers, like the presence of authority figures, may cause you to
feel less personally responsible for intervening.
If, however, you assume responsibility for helping, a fourth decision you must make is the appropriate form
of assistance to render. But in the heat of the moment, what if you are not sure what to do? You may
become paralyzed with uncertainty about exactly how to render assistance.
Finally, if you notice something unusual, interpret it as an emergency, assume responsibility, and decide
how to help, you still must decide whether to implement your course of prosocial action.
Reading
·  Franzoi, S. (2003). Social Psychology. Boston: McGraw-Hill. Chapter 14.
Other Readings
·  Lord, C.G. (1997). Social Psychology. Orlando: Harcourt Brace and Company. Chapter 8.
·  David G. Myers, D. G. (2002). Social Psychology (7th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
·  Taylor, S.E. (2006). Social Psychology (12th ed.). New York: Prentice Hall.
156
Table of Contents:
  1. INTRODUCTION TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY:Readings, Main Elements of Definitions
  2. INTRODUCTION TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY:Social Psychology and Sociology
  3. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY:Scientific Method
  4. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY:Evaluate Ethics
  5. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH PROCESS, DESIGNS AND METHODS (CONTINUED)
  6. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OBSERVATIONAL METHOD
  7. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CORRELATIONAL METHOD:
  8. CONDUCTING RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY EXPERIMENTAL METHOD
  9. THE SELF:Meta Analysis, THE INTERNET, BRAIN-IMAGING TECHNIQUES
  10. THE SELF (CONTINUED):Development of Self awareness, SELF REGULATION
  11. THE SELF (CONTINUE…….):Journal Activity, POSSIBLE HISTORICAL EFFECTS
  12. THE SELF (CONTINUE……….):SELF-SCHEMAS, SELF-COMPLEXITY
  13. PERSON PERCEPTION:Impression Formation, Facial Expressions
  14. PERSON PERCEPTION (CONTINUE…..):GENDER SOCIALIZATION, Integrating Impressions
  15. PERSON PERCEPTION: WHEN PERSON PERCEPTION IS MOST CHALLENGING
  16. ATTRIBUTION:The locus of causality, Stability & Controllability
  17. ATTRIBUTION ERRORS:Biases in Attribution, Cultural differences
  18. SOCIAL COGNITION:We are categorizing creatures, Developing Schemas
  19. SOCIAL COGNITION (CONTINUE…….):Counterfactual Thinking, Confirmation bias
  20. ATTITUDES:Affective component, Behavioral component, Cognitive component
  21. ATTITUDE FORMATION:Classical conditioning, Subliminal conditioning
  22. ATTITUDE AND BEHAVIOR:Theory of planned behavior, Attitude strength
  23. ATTITUDE CHANGE:Factors affecting dissonance, Likeability
  24. ATTITUDE CHANGE (CONTINUE……….):Attitudinal Inoculation, Audience Variables
  25. PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION:Activity on Cognitive Dissonance, Categorization
  26. PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION (CONTINUE……….):Religion, Stereotype threat
  27. REDUCING PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION:The contact hypothesis
  28. INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION:Reasons for affiliation, Theory of Social exchange
  29. INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION (CONTINUE……..):Physical attractiveness
  30. INTIMATE RELATIONSHIPS:Applied Social Psychology Lab
  31. SOCIAL INFLUENCE:Attachment styles & Friendship, SOCIAL INTERACTIONS
  32. SOCIAL INFLUENCE (CONTINE………):Normative influence, Informational influence
  33. SOCIAL INFLUENCE (CONTINUE……):Crimes of Obedience, Predictions
  34. AGGRESSION:Identifying Aggression, Instrumental aggression
  35. AGGRESSION (CONTINUE……):The Cognitive-Neo-associationist Model
  36. REDUCING AGGRESSION:Punishment, Incompatible response strategy
  37. PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR:Types of Helping, Reciprocal helping, Norm of responsibility
  38. PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR (CONTINUE………):Bystander Intervention, Diffusion of responsibility
  39. GROUP BEHAVIOR:Applied Social Psychology Lab, Basic Features of Groups
  40. GROUP BEHAVIOR (CONTINUE…………):Social Loafing, Deindividuation
  41. up Decision GROUP BEHAVIOR (CONTINUE……….):GroProcess, Group Polarization
  42. INTERPERSONAL POWER: LEADERSHIP, The Situational Perspective, Information power
  43. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY APPLIED: SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY IN COURT
  44. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY APPLIED: SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY IN CLINIC
  45. FINAL REVIEW:Social Psychology and related fields, History, Social cognition