|
|||||
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
LESSON
05
HISTORY OF
CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY
ROOTS OF
RESEARCH & ASSESSMENT IN CLINICAL
PSYCHOLOGY
The
evolution of the field of clinical
psychology after Lightner
Witmer can be best understood
through
an
examination of how clinical psychologists
came to be involved in each of
four different
activities:
1.
Research,
2.
Assessment,
3.
Treatment,
and
4.
Prevention.
Clinical
psychologists became involved in these
endeavors at different points
during the twentieth
century
and for very different
reasons. It is important to comprehend the
role of clinical psychologists in
these
four activities both to understand
forces in this field's past
and to anticipate changes in its
future.
Likewise,
it is important to possess an
understanding not only of the
events that shaped
clinical
psychology,
but also of the broader social context in
which the field has
developed.
During
the early years, clinical
psychology was a science and
profession dominated by males
(Snyder,
McDermott,
Leibowitz, & Cheavens, 2000).
For example, in 1917, only
13 percent of APA members
were
women. Although women made
progress in the fields of developmental
and school psychology,
from
1920 through 1974 women
comprised only 24 percent of graduates
with doctoral degrees
in
clinical
psychology (Snyder et
al.).
By
1994, however, 58.7 percent of
students admitted to doctoral
programs in clinical psychology
were
women.
Thus, the composition of clinical
psychology has changed from
being overwhelmingly male to
being
overwhelmingly female.
1.
RESEARCH
HOW
CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGISTS BECAME
INVOLVED IN RESEARCH
Witmer
and the other founders of clinical
psychology were researchers who were
interested in the
application
of their research to the benefit of
others.
Clinical
researchers try to add to these bodies of
knowledge both to increase
their understanding of
psychopathology,
illness, and health and to improve
their methods for its
treatment and prevention.
Because
of their broad training in
basic behavioral science,
clinical psychologists are able to
draw
conclusions
and contribute to research in a variety
of different areas and to collaborate
with
professionals
from other
disciplines.
THE
SCOPE OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGICAL
RESEARCH
Clinical
psychological research has
steadily grown in its scope
since the early 1900s. This
subfield now
includes:
Research
on the basic characteristics and prevalence of
psychopathology. (epidemiology),
the
causes of psychopathology
(etiology),
the
measurement of behavior and psychological
characteristics of individuals
(assessment),
the
role of the brain and central nervous
system (clinical
Neuropsychology),
27
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
the
treatment of psychopathology
(psychotherapy),
The
prevention of psychopathology and the
promotion of psychological health, and
the links
between
psychological factors and physical
health and illness (health
psychology/behavioral
medicine).
There
has been landmark research
in each of these areas
during the past 70 years, the
results of which
led
to substantial changes in knowledge about
a particular problem or
issue.
RESEARCHES
THAT SHAPED THE
FIELD
Clinical
psychology has been shaped
not only by findings from
research studies but also by
important
reviews
of research evidence and by the
development of new methods
for clinical practice.
Two
examples are particularly
prominent in this
regard:
1)
Effectiveness of Psychotherapy
2)
Statistical vs. Clinical
Prediction
1)
EFFECTIVENESS OF PSYCHOTHERAPY
In
the early 1950s, the field of
psychotherapy was in its early
stages of development, and much of
the
practice
of psychotherapy was based on the
psychoanalytic model developed by
Freud.
Research
on the effectiveness of this approach to psychotherapy
was very limited,
however-most
practitioners
simply assumed that the
methods they were using were
effective in treating their
patients.
Given
this widespread acceptance of the
belief that psychotherapy
was effective, a paper published
in
1952
by British psychologist Hans Eysenck
created enormous
controversy.
Eysenck
argued that there was little or no
evidence that psychotherapy was
any more effective than
no
treatment
at all.
He
reached this conclusion by
comparing (two sources of data:
the results of 24
studies
that had been
conducted
on the outcomes of psychotherapy, and
information on rates of recovery
from emotional
distress
in the absence of treatment, or what is
referred to as spontaneous
remission.
Eysenck
reported that treated individuals
actually did worse than
did people who received
no
psychotherapy:
Whereas 72
percent
of the individuals who did
not receive treatment recovered
from
their
problems, only 44
percent
of those receiving psychoanalysis and 64 percent of
those receiving
"eclectic"
psychotherapy recovered.
Although
the rates of improvement presented by
Eysenck are much lower than
those found in many
studies
that have been published
since the appearance of his
paper, his report had a
significant impact on
the
field.
Eysenck
challenged clinical psychologists and
other mental health professionals to
provide better
evidence
for the effectiveness of their treatment
methods.
Initiated
by Eysenck's paper, more and better
research on the effects of psychotherapy
has been
conducted,
leading to more effective methods of
treatment and a better understanding of
how and why
psychotherapy
works. The current evidence on the
effects of psychotherapy is much more
positive than
the
perspective offered by Eysenck 50 years
ago.
28
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
2)
Statistical vs. Clinical
Prediction
A
second example of research
that changed the field is a short
but important book;
"Statistical Versus
Clinical
Prediction" published by psychologist
Paul Meehl in 1954 that had a
significant impact on
psychological
testing and assessment. Before
Meehl published his book,
psychologists relied heavily on
their
subjective judgments and intuitions in
interpreting the results of psychological
tests.
This
approach, referred to as clinical
judgment or clinical prediction,
was based on the assumption
that
clinical
psychologists learn a unique set of
skills that allows them to make
accurate judgments about
people
and to predict such things as patients'
ability to benefit from
psychotherapy, people's potential
for
success
in a job, or the likely course of
individuals' psychological
problems.
Meehl
challenged these assumptions by
demonstrating that judgments based on
statistical data
representing
patterns of behavior in large
samples of people provide a more
accurate basis for
making
judgments
and predictions about specific
individuals than do the subjective
judgments of single
clinicians.
The
findings reported by Meehl in
1954 still hold true
today (Dawes, Faust, & Meehl,
1989; Meehl,
1997),
statistically based predictions
are still more accurate than
clinical judgment.
CONTRIBUTION
OF RESEARCH IN CLINICAL
PSYCHOLOGY
Although
single groundbreaking studies and
commentaries have clearly important
effects on the field of
clinical
psychology, they are
relatively rare and do not
represent how most of the
growth and
development
in the field occurs. Rather, the
greatest contribution of research in
clinical psychology is
the
slow and gradual accumulation of
knowledge that comes from
the results of dozens of studies on
a
particular
topic.
For
example, procedures for the treatment of
anxiety disorders have been developed
through the efforts
of
a large number of different researchers
who have conducted many series of
carefully designed studies
using
a wide variety of research
methods (Barlow,
1998).
These
procedures include the treatment of
generalized anxiety disorder,
panic disorder,
post-traumatic
stress
disorder, and specific phobias.
One of the most striking
features of research in clinical
psychology
today
is the breadth of topics that are
included within the
field.
Throughout
much of the first century of
clinical psychology, three topic
areas have been focal points
of
research:
·
The
nature and etiology of
psychopathology;
·
The
reliability and validity of methods of
psychological assessment, especially
psychological
tests;
and
·
Psychotherapy
efficacy (whether or not psychotherapy
can work) and effectiveness
(whether
psychotherapy
actually does work in
practice).
RESEARCH
IN CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY TODAY
Today,
however, research in clinical
psychology extends well
beyond these core topics of
research.
Areas
that clinical psychologists now
investigate include:
·
The
role of psychological factors in the
development of physical disease
(e.g., cancer, heart
disease),
·
The
relative effectiveness of psychotherapy as compared
with medication in the treatment
of
psychopathology,
·
The
prevention of violent
behavior,
29
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
·
The
long-term consequences of sexual assault,
harassment and rape, and many,
many other
topics.
2.
ASSESSMENT
CLINICAL
PSYCHOLOGISTS INVOLVEMENT IN
ASSESSMENT
Since
its inception, psychology
has been uniquely concerned
with the measurement of
differences
between
individuals on important cognitive and
personality characteristics. The study of
differences
between
individuals on psychological tests and
measurements began with the
work of Sir Francis
Galton
in
England in the late
1800s.
Galton
was fascinated by the work of his cousin
Charles Darwin on differences in
characteristics both
between
and within species, and in the
process of natural selection that is
influenced by these
differences.
Galton focused on the concept of
individual differences between people,
especially in
various
aspects of perception and menial
abilities.
STUDY
OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES
Early
interest in individual difference testing
in the United States is marked by the
work of James
McKeen
Catell at the University of Pennsylvania.
Trained in Wundt's laboratory in Germany
and
influenced
by a meeting with Galton in
England, Catell constructed
tests to measure various
facets of
sensori-motor
functioning.
As
a result of these early
influences, one strong thread through the
history of clinical psychology is
the
development
of tests and other procedures to
assess and measure characteristics of
individuals.
THE
INFLUENCE OF BINET'S INTELLIGENCE
TEST
BACK
GROUND TO BINET'S INTELLIGENCE
TEST
Around
the time that Witmer was
developing an application of psychology
to help children who
were
experiencing
difficulties learning in school, events
in Europe were also leading to the
development of
methods
to measure children's potential
for learning.
In
1904, the Minister of Public
Instruction in Paris wanted to
ensure that children with
limited
intellectual
skills were still provided
with an education. Alfred
Binet and Theodore Simon
were
commissioned
by the French government to develop a
tool to aid in decisions about the
appropriate
educational
programs for French
schoolchildren.
Binet
was a French researcher trained in
both law and medicine. In
order to study individual
differences,
he
felt it was necessary to
sample a wide range of
complex intellectual processes so
that the spread of
scores
obtained by different individuals
would be broad (Reisman, 1976).
Binet's
work resulted in the first formal
test of intelligence, the 1905
Binet-Simon scale, consisting
of
30
items of increasing difficulty.
By
1908 this original simple
test had been expanded into an
instrument composed of 59 tests
grouped at
age
levels from three to thirteen
years according to the percentage of
children of a particular age
who
passed
a given item (Reisman).
Interest
in Binet's work grew over
the next few years, and versions of the
Binet-Simon scale were
imported
to the United States. The
version that eventually
became the accepted U.S.
translation and
standardization
of the Binet-Simon scale (the
Stanford-Bind
Intelligence Test) was
developed by
psychologist
Louis Terman of Stanford
University in 1916.
30
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
WORLD
WAR I; A TEST FOR CLINICAL
PSYCHOLOGY
As
the United States prepared to enter the
war that was raging in
Europe in 1917, the American
military
was
faced with an unprecedented task: the conscription and
creation of a massive army and
navy. There
was
an enormous need to evaluate
quickly and accurately the qualifications
of over 1 million young
men
as
potential members of the armed
forces (Driskell & Olmstead,
1989).
Physicians
were enlisted in the task of conducting
physical evaluations of these
draftees to determine
whether
they were physically fit to
serve during the war. But
the military recognized the need to
also
evaluate
the mental and intellectual
qualifications of these potential
soldiers. Physicians could not
fill
this
role, because the evaluation of
mental functioning was not
within their realm of
expertise.
Based
on their knowledge of human learning and
memory and the measurement of
individual
differences
in human intelligence, psychologists were called on to
fill this role. In 1917, a
group of
psychologists,
headed by APA president Robert Yerkes,
undertook the task of developing
tools to
measure
the mental abilities of future soldiers
(Driskell & Olmstcad, 1989).
The
psychological tests that were
available and in use at the
time (e.g., the test developed by
Binet and
Simon)
required individual administration.
Consequently, these tests were
impractical for use with
the
large
number of recruits involved in the military.
Therefore, Yerkes and his colleagues
set about the task
of
developing a quick and efficient
test of intelligence that
could be administered to large groups
of
individuals
simultaneously.
There
effort yielded two
tests,
1.
The Army
Alpha (a test of
verbal skills) and
2.
The Army
Beta (a test of
nonverbal skills).
The
enduring consequence of this
work is that it established psychologists as experts
in the
measurement
of individual characteristics in ways
that were practical and useful.
This opportunity for
psychology
to contribute to the war through the
application of psychological tests
increased the status
and
visibility of psychologists and of psychological
testing.
It
is unlikely that this first
large-scale application of scientific
psychological knowledge and
methods
would
have occurred without strong pressure
from external sources, in
this case the U.S.
military.
ASSESSMENT
AFTER WORLD WAR I
Following
World War I, clinical psychologists
became well known for
their testing skills. A
testing
development
occurred, such that by 1940
over 500 psychological tests
had been produced.
These
tests included both verbal
and non verbal intelligence
tests, career interest, personality
and
vocational
skills tests. Tests were available
for children of all ages and
abilities as well as for
adults.
ADVANCES
IN PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING AND
ASSESSMENT
In
addition to the powerful social forces
that led clinical psychology
to become involved in
psychological
assessment and testing, significant
advances in research have also
played an important
role.
For example, the publication of the
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
Inventory (MMPI) by
psychologist
Starke Hathaway in 1943 represented a
major change in the way that
psychologists
measured
personality and
psychopathology.
The
MMPI relies on statistical comparisons of
the test responses of an individual to
those of a large
sample
of other people who have
already been tested. These
comparisons are used to
determine the
degree
to which the individual is similar to a
group of people with known
personality characteristics, or
31
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
people
with a specific type of
psychopathology. Thus, the MMPI represented an
important shift away
from
the more clinical, subjective approach to
assessment and toward a more
statistical, empirically
based
method of assessment.
Another
important advance in assessment occurred
during the 1960s with the
recognition that
direct
observations
of people's behavior might represent an
important source of information,
perhaps more
valid
than relying on their
responses to psychological
tests.
The
first applications of behavioral
observation as a means of assessment were
conducted in schools and
psychiatric
hospitals, settings in which it was rather
easy for a psychologist to
observe an individual's
behavior
and in which the environment was
relatively contained and
controlled.
For
example, Bijou, Peterson,
Harris, Alien, and Johnston (1969)
described a method for the
experimental
study of young children in
natural settings, including their home,
school, and other
institutions,
as well as the behavior of parents,
peers, and professional
workers.
In
general, research has shown that
behavioral observations can be conducted in a
manner that is
reliable
(different raters independently
generate similar ratings of the
same individual) and that
these
observations
can be useful in formulating
and evaluating the effects of
treatment.
CONCLUSION
If
the development of clinical psychology
had followed the path set by
Witmer, Terman, and others
in
the
early 1900s, it would have
slowly emerged as a field
that was based on the
careful application of the
young
science of psychology. This is
not what happened, however.
Indeed, much of the rest of
the
history
of the field is marked by decisions made by
psychologists to move into new
areas and new
applications
even though the scientific knowledge in
these areas may not have
been sufficient to
warrant
such
an application.
There
were often powerful social forces
pressing psychologists to step forward to
address an important
issue
or assume an important role.
The results of these decisions have
been far-reaching, because
the
field
of clinical psychology has expanded at a
rate that has at times challenged
its scientific
knowledge
base
and expertise.
32
Table of Contents:
|
|||||