|
|||||
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Lecture
33
ORIGINS AND
TRADITIONAL TECHNIQUES OF BEHAVIOR
THERAPY
Traditionally,
the behavioral approach
allies itself with
(1)
A scientific emphasis
and
(2)
A deemphasis of the role of
inferred variables.
The
behaviorists are likely to trace
their origins to the
"science" of Skinner or Pavlov
rather than the
"rnentalism"
of Freud. The focus is on
stimuli and responses rather
than variables that are presumed
to
mediate-them. However, behavior
therapy over the years has
broadened its scope to include
techniques that
address
cognitive and other mediation
processes (Goldfried & Davison,
1994). Nevertheless, it is
instructive
to
review behavior therapy's
historical roots.
A
BRIEF HISTORY:
Beginning
by presenting the groundbreaking
work of Watson and Rayner
(1920), who conducted
the
widely
cited laboratory study of Albert and
the laboratory rat. This study
was in
effect, a
demonstration of
how
a "neurosis" can develop in a child. In
the tradition of Pavlovian
conditioning, Albert was
given a
laboratory
rat to play with. But each
time the rat was introduced,
loud noise was introduced
simultaneously.
After
a few such trials, the rat
(previously a neutral stimulus) elicited a
fearful response that also
generalized
to
similar furry
objects.
Mary
Cover Jones (1924) demonstrated
how such
learned
fears can be removed. A
3-year-old boy,-Peter,
was
afraid of rabbits, rats,-and
other such objects. To eradicate
the fear, Jones brought a
caged rabbit closer
and
closer as the boy was
eating. The feared object thus
became associated with food,
and after a
few
months Peter's fear of the
rabbit disappeared entirely. It is
important, however, to recall
Jones's
admonition
that the fear of the rabbit
must not be so intense that
the child will develop an
aversion to food.
Watson's
conditioning of fears and
Jones's 'reconditioning" of them
were erect antecedents of
the
development
of Wolpe's (1958) therapy by reciprocal
inhibition, which arrived on
the scene some 30
years
later.
As
the foregoing experiences of
Albert and Peter suggest,
the major theoretical underpinnings of
the
behavior
therapy movement were Pavlovian
conditioning and Hullian
learning theory. In the
1950s. Joseph
Wolpe
and Arnold Lazarus in South
Africa and Hans Eysenck at
Maudsley Hospital in London
began to
apply
the results of animal
research to the acquisition
and elimination of anxiety in
humans. Wolpe began
to
experiment
with the reduction of fears in
humans by having patients,
while in a state of heightened
relaxation,
imagine the situations in which
their tears occurred.
Wolpe's technique of systematic
desensitization,
like Jones's reconditioning
work, provided a practical demonstration
of how principles of
learning
could be applied in the
clinical setting. In his work on
conditioned reflex therapy,
Salter (1949)
also
attempted to develop a method of therapy
that was derived from the
Pavlovian tradition.
It
is important to note that these investigators
did not merely introduce new
techniques. They also
argued
vigorously
that their techniques were
derived from the framework
of a systematic experimental science.
In
addition,
they took pains to point
out that their demonstrations of
the origins and treatment of
neurotic fears
proved
that it was unnecessary to subscribe to
the "mentalistic demonology" of
Freudianism or to the
"psychiatric
pigeonholing" practiced by
Kraepelinians
At
about the same time that
Wolpe, Lazarus, and Eysenck
were developing their
conditioning procedures,
the
operant tradition was
beginning to have an impact. Skinner
and his colleagues (Lindsley &
Skinner,
1954;
Skinner, 1953) were
demonstrating that the behavior of
hospitalized psychotic patients could
be
modified
by operant procedures. By establishing
controlled environments to ensure that certain
responses of
the
patient would be followed by specific
consequences, significant behavioral
changes were
produced.
At
first, there was a radical
quality to behavior therapy .The inner
world of the patient was
largely ignored
in
the rush to focus on
behavior. Whether in reaction to the
mentalism of psychoanalysis or out of an
overly
243
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
provincial
view of what should be the
subject matter of science,
the early behavior
therapists studiously
avoided
anything of a cognitive
nature.
However,
in 1954, Julian Rotter published his book
Social
Learning and Clinical Psychology.
In
it he
demonstrated
convincingly that a motivation reinforcement
approach to psychology could be coupled
with
a
cognitive-expectancy approach. Thus,
behavior was regarded as
being determined both by the
value of
reinforcements
and by the expectancy that
such reinforcements would
occur following the behavior
in
question.
What is more, Rotter's novel
views were supported by a
series of laboratory studies that
left no
doubt
that one could be clinical, oriented
toward both learning theory
and cognitive theory,
and
scientifically
respectable, all at the same
time.
Also
significant in this context was the
application of Albert Bandura's
(1969) social learning
contributions
to
e modification of a behavior. It was
theorists such as Rotter and Bandura
who led the way to
the current
cognitive
emphasis, giving behavior therapy a
wider ranging and
serviceable character.
It
is important to point out that
the "mentalism" of psychoanalysis or
other psychodynamic approaches is
not
the same as the "cognitive
processes" concepts that are
used today. Freud's
references to thinking
processes
were never defined operationally.
They were vague notions incapable of
objective measurement,
poorly
anchored either to antecedent conditions or
consequent outcomes. More
often than not, Freud
viewed
thinking processes as irrational,
distorting processes rather
than problem-solving
processes.
For
Freud, mentalism seemed to function
largely in the service of
the reified ego, id,
and superego----little
people
who ran about the mind
distorting, projecting, condemning, or
figuring out ways of fooling
one
another.
In contrast, current notions of cognition
emphasize such concepts as
expectancies, cognitive
schemas,
or memory processes. These are
concepts that can be measured
and quantified. They can be
objec-
tively
defined in ways that lead to reliable
understanding among separate-
investigators.
TRADITIONAL
TECHNIQUES OF BEHAVIORAL
THERAPY:
Behavior
therapists use a variety of specific
techniques-not only for
different patients but for
the same
patient
at different points in the overall
treatment process. Lazarus (1971a)
refers to this as
a broad
spectrum
behavior therapy. Each
technique can serve a specific purpose
but that, in reality, they
are
complementary.
For
example, a woman who has trouble
coping with a domineering
husband may undergo assertiveness
training
to team specific behaviors. But when she
uses these behaviors, other sets of
fears about their
relationship
may begin to worry her. Therefore,
she may also require therapeutic sessions
that will help her
restructure
her beliefs about the marriage that
are illogical and tend to
perpetuate her submissive
behavior.
She
might also participate in modeling or
observational learning to help her
cope.
A
comprehensive behavioral assessment is
conducted before behavioral treatments or
techniques are
selected
and implemented-.-For
example, a functional analysis of the
presenting problem helps to
identify
(1)
The stimulus or antecedent conditions that
bring on
the
problematic behavior;
(2)
The Organismic variables (such as
cognitive biases) that are related to
the problematic behavior;
(3)
The exact description of the
problem; and
(4)
The consequences of the problematic
behavior.
By
completing such a detailed analysis,
behavior and cognitive-behavioral
therapists can prescribe
ap-
propriate
treatments.
Now
we will go through some
behavioral techniques.
244
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
SYSTEMATIC
DESENSITIZATION:
This
technique is typically applied when a patient
has the capacity to respond adequately to
a particular
situation
(or class of situations), yet
reacts with anxiety, fear,
or avoidance. Basically,
systematic
desensitization
is a technique to reduce anxiety. Developed by
Salter (1949) and Wolpe
(1958), it is based
on
reciprocal
inhibition the
apparently simple principle that one
cannot be relaxed and anxious
simultaneously.
The idea is to teach Patients to relax
and then, while they
are in a relaxed state, to'
introduce
a
gradually increasing series of
anxiety-producing stimuli. Eventually,
the patient becomes desensitized
to
the
feared stimuli by virtue of
having experienced them in a relaxed
state. Systematic desensitization
has
been
shown to be efficacious for animal
phobias, public speaking
anxiety, and social anxiety
(Chambless et
al.,
1998).
TECHNIQUES
AND PROCEDURES:
Systematic
desensitization begins with
the collection of a history of
the patient's problem. This
includes
information
both about specific precipitating conditions
and about developmental factors.
Collecting a his-
tory
may require several interviews, and it
often includes the administration of
questionnaires. The principal
reason
for all of this is to pinpoint
the locus of the patient's
anxiety. It is also part of assessment to
determine
whether
systematic desensitization is the proper
treatment. In a patient with adequate
coping potential who
nevertheless
reacts to certain situations with severe
anxiety, desensitization is often
appropriate.
On
the other hand, if a patient lacks
certain skills and then
becomes anxious in situations that require
those
skills,
desensitization could be inappropriate
and counterproductive, For example, if a
man becomes
seriously
anxious in social situations that involve dancing, it
would seem more efficient to
see that he learns
to
dance rather than desensitize
him to what is, in fact, a
behavioral deficit.
Next,
the problem is explained to
the patient. This explanation is
normally elaborated to include
examples
from
the patient's life and to
cover the manner in which
the patient acquired and
maintains the anxieties.
Following
this, the rationale for
systematic desensitization is also
explained. The explanations and
the
illustrations
should be in language that the patient
can understand-free from
scientific jargon. In a sense,
the
clinician
uses this phase to "sell"
the patient on the efficacy of
systematic desensitization. It should be
added
that
the entire process of interviewing,
assessment, and explanation is
conducted with warmth,
acceptance,
and
understanding.
The
next two phases involve
training
in relaxation and
the establishment of an
anxiety
hierarchy. While
work
is begun on the anxiety
hierarchy, training in relaxation is
also started.
Relaxation:
Behavior
therapists frequently use
the progressive relaxation
methods of Jacobson (1938). Te
patient is first
taught
to tense and relax
particular muscle groups and
then to distinguish between
sensations of relaxation
and
tensing. The instructions for
relaxation can easily be taped
and played at home for
practice. Generally,
about
six sessions are devoted to
relaxation training. In some
instances, hypnosis may be
used to induce
relaxation.
In other instances, the patient may be
asked to imagine relaxing
scene still other
instances,
breathing
exercises are used to
enhance relaxation.
The
Anxiety Hierarchy:
In
discussions about specific problems, the
situations in which they occur,
and their development,
the
patient
and the therapist work
together to construct a hierarchy. The
recurrent themes in the
patient's diffi-
culties
and anxieties are isolated and then
ordered in terms of their power to induce
anxiety (from situations
that
provoke very low levels of
anxiety through situations that precipitate
extreme anxiety reactions).
A
typical
anxiety hierarchy consists of 20 to 25
items in approximately equal intervals
from low through
245
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
moderate
to extreme. The following
anxiety hierarchy was that of a 24-year
old female student
who
experienced
severe examination anxiety
(Wolpe, 1973):
1.
Four days before an
examination.
2.
Three days before an
examination.
3.
Two days before an
examination.
4.
One day before an
examination.
5.
The night before an
examination.
6.
The examination paper lies
face down before her.
7.
Awaiting the distribution of
examination papers.
8.
before the unopened doors of
the examination room.
9.
In the process of answering an
examination paper.
10.
on the way to the university
on the day of the
examination.
This
hierarchy illustrates two points: First, it is
organized largely along spatial-temporal
lines. Second, the
items
are not exactly organized in
a logical fashion.
One
might expect item 10 (the
most anxiety-provoking item) to be
placed near the middle of
the hierarchy.
This
suggests how idiosyncratic
hierarchies
can
be-after all, it is the patient's
anxiety, not the
clinician's!
In
the desensitization procedure,
the patient is asked to imagine
the weakest item in the
hierarchy (the item
that
provokes the least anxiety)
while being completely relaxed.
The therapist describes the
scene, and the
patient
imagines (for about 10 seconds)
being in the scene. Therapist
moves the patient up the
hierarchy
gradually
(between two and five
items per session). However,
if at any time the level of
anxiety begins to
increase,
the patient is instructed to signal,
where upon the therapist
requests that the patient stop
visualizing
that
scene. The therapist then
helps the patient to relax
once more. After a few
minutes, the procedure
can
be
started again. Ideally, over a
period of several sessions,
the patient will be able to
imagine the highest
item
in the hierarchy without
discomfort.
Rationale:
Although
Wolpe's explanation for the
success of systematic desensitization is
based on the principle
of
counter
conditioning (the substitution of
relaxation for anxiety),
others are not so sure
(Davison & Wilson,
1973).
Some have argued that the
operative process is really extinction.
That
is, when the patient
repeatedly
visualizes
anxiety-generating situations but without
ensuing bad experiences, the
anxiety responses are
eventually
extinguished (Wilson & Davison, 1971).
Alternatively, Mathews (1971)
argues on behalf of a
habituation
hypothesis.
Emmelkamp (1982) has reviewed
the empirical support for
these and other
theoretical
explanations.
The
standard method of desensitization is to
present scenes in a graduated
ascending fashion in order to
avoid
premature arousal of anxiety that
would disrupt the procedure.
However, some clinician have
found
that
presenting the
hierarchy
in the reverse order (most
anxiety-provoking items first) is
also effective in
reducing
various phobias. Richardson and
Suinn (1973) also report
positive results when participants
are
exposed
only to the three highest
hierarchy scenes.
Systematic
desensitization involves a number of
components. The instructions suggest that a
positive
outcome
is likely. Consequently, the Patient's
expectations for improvement may affect
the process.
Another
crucial element may be positive reinforcement
from the therapist follow,
in, the patient's reports
of
lessened
anxiety, improvement outside the
consulting room, or the successful
completion of anxiety
hierarchies.
For
example, Leitenberg, Agras, Barlow, and
Oliveau (1969) observed that,
with snake phobias, the
effects
of
systematic desensitization are
best when the therapist uses
reinforcing comments, such as
"Good,"
"Excellent,"
and "You're doing fine,"
when participants (1) visualize a scene
without reporting anxiety,
(2)
complete
a hierarchy item, and (3) report
progress in approaching a snake during
practice. Goldfried
(1971)
246
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
argues
that systematic desensitization is far
from a passive process that is
applied to patients to reduce
their
fears.
Rather, it represents the acquisition of
a skill that the patients
can use to reduce their
own fear.
In
that sense, Goldfried regards
systematic desensitization as training in
self control. From a cognitive
viewpoint,
Valins and Ray (1967)
explain the effectiveness of
systematic desensitization in terms
of
patients'
belief that they are relaxed.
Others, such as Sullivan and
Denney (1977), emphasize the
importance
of
getting the patient to expect
improvement.
All
of the foregoing suggests that
systematic desensitization is hardly
the simple mechanical or
conditioning
process
that it was once thought to
be. A number of relationship variables
seem implicated, as well
as
beliefs
or expectations--on the part of the
patient. In general, systematic
desensitization has proven to be
a
moderately
effective form of psychological
intervention for a variety of
clinical conditions. As might be
ex-
pected,
research suggests that it is most
effective when used to treat
anxiety disorders particularly
specific
phobias.
Social anxiety, public speaking
anxiety, and generalized
anxiety disorder.
Exposure
Therapy:
The
term exposure
therapy is used
to describe a behavior therapy technique that is a
refinement of a set of
procedures
originally known as flooding or
implosion. The roots of
exposure therapy can be traced
to
Masserman
(1943), who studied anxiety
reactions and avoidance behaviors in
cats. Masserman's studies
in-
volved
inducing "neurotic behaviors" in cats by
administering shock under certain
environmental
conditions.
He subsequently discovered that the
avoidance behavior could be extinguished
if the cats were
forced
to remain in the situation in
which they had previously
been shocked (that is, no
escape or avoidance
was
possible). These 'findings
were the basis for
developing anxiety treatments
for humans There is
empirical
support for the efficacy of
exposure treatments for specific
phobias, panic disorder, agoraphobia,,
social
phobia, post traumatic stress disorder,
and obsessive-compulsive disorder.
In
exposure therapy, patients
expose themselves to those
stimuli or situations that were
previously feared
and
avoided. The "exposure" can
be in real life (in vivo)
or
in fantasy (in-imagino). In the
latter version,
patients
are asked to imagine
themselves in the presence of
the feared stimulus (such as
a spider) or in the
anxiety-provoking
situation (such as speaking in
front of an audience). Several
researchers suggest that
certain
features must be present in
exposure treatments in order for
the patient to achieve maximum
benefit
(Barlow
& Cerny, 1988):
1.
Exposure should be of long rather than
short duration.
2.
Exposure should be repeated until
all fear/anxiety is
eliminated.
3.
Exposure should be graduated, starting
with low-anxiety stimuli/situations
and progressing to
high-
anxiety
stimuli/situations.
4.
Patients must attend to the
feared stimulus and interact
with it as much as
possible.
5.
Exposure must provoke
anxiety.
Like
the other behavioral therapies
exposure treatment can be
used as a self-contained treatment or as
one
component
of a multimodal treatment.
What
is especially ingenious about their version of
exposure treatment is that they
have patients expose
themselves
to interoceptive
cues---internal
physiological stimuli such as
rapid breathing and dizziness.
This
modification
was necessary because
individuals suffering from panic disorder
typically report that their
panic
attacks are unpredictable and
"come out of the blue." In
such cases, no external anxiety
provoking
stimulus
or situation is apparent. In contrast,
individuals with other, non-panic
anxiety disorders report
acute
anxiety
primarily in the face of
certain external stimuli or
situations.
247
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Researchers
recently compared the effectiveness of
two forms of treatment for
panic disorder with
agoraphobia:
one that included interoceptive
exposure and one that incorporated
breathing retraining
instead
of interoceptive exposure. Although
both forms of treatment were
effective, results indicated that
panic
disorder patients who received
the interoceptive exposure
component reported less impairment
and
fewer
panic attacks at post treatment
and at follow-up. Thus, the
addition of the interoceptive
exposure
component
had beneficial
effects.
Behavior
Rehearsal:
Included
under this broad heading are a variety of
techniques whose aim is to
enlarge the patient's
repertoire
of
coping behaviors. Clearly, behavior
rehearsal is not a new
concept, it has been around in
one form or
another
for many years. For example,
Moreno (1947 developed psychodrama, a
form of role playing,
to
help
solve patients' problems, and
Kelly (1955) used fixed-role
therapy.
However,
it is important to note that such
forms of role playing or
behavior rehearsal have
purposes that
depart
from behavioral goals. For
Moreno, role playing
provided a therapeutic release of
emotions that was
also
diagnostic-in identifying the
causes of the patient's
problems. For Kelly, role
playing was a method
of
altering
the patient's cognitive
structure. Again, we are reminded that
specific therapeutic techniques are
not
the
exclusive province of one theoretical
frame of reference. Different
theorists may use similar
techniques
for
vastly different
reasons.
The
Technique:
According
to Goldfried and Davison
(1994), the use of behavior
rehearsal involves four
stages.
The
first stage is to prepare
the patient by explaining the
necessity for acquiring new behaviors,
getting the
patient
to accept for rehearsal as a useful
device, and reducing any initial
anxiety over the prospect of
role
playing.
The
second stage involves the
selection of target situations. At this
point many therapists draw up a
hierarchy
of role playing or rehearsal situations.
This hierarchy should relate directly to
those situations in
which
the patient has been having
difficulty. A sample hierarchy of target
situations (ranked in order of the
increasingly
complex behavioral skills required) might
be as follows:
1.
You ask a secretary for
information about a class.
2.
You ask a student in class
about last week's
assignment.
3.
After class, you approach
the instructor with a question about
the lecture.
4.
You go to the instructor's
office and engage her in
conversation about a certain
point.
5.
You purposely engage another
student, who you know
disagrees with you, in a
minor debate about
some
issue.
The
third stage is the actual
behavior rehearsal. Moving up
the hierarchy, the patient plays
the appropriate
roles,
with the therapist providing
both coaching and feedback
regarding the adequacy of the
patient's
performance.
Sometimes videotaped replays are
used as, an aid. In other
instances the "therapist (or
a
therapeutic
aide) exchanges roles with the patient in
order to provide an appropriate model. When
patients
develop
proficiency in one target situation,
they move up the
hierarchy.
The
final stage is the patient's
actual utilization of newly
acquired skills in real-life situations.
After such in
vivo
experiences, the patient and the
therapist discuss the
patient's performance and feelings about
the
experiences.
Sometimes patients are asked
to keep the records describing
the situations they were in,
their
behavior,
and its consequences.
248
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Assertiveness
Training:
One
application of behavioral rehearsal is
assertiveness
training. Wolpe regarded
assertive responses as an
example
of how reciprocal inhibition works. That
is, it is impossible to behave assertively
and to be passive
simultaneously.
Situations that once evoked anxiety will
no _longer
do so because the assertive
behavior
inhibits
the anxiety
Originally,
assertiveness training was
designed as a treatment for
persons whose anxiety seemed
to stem
from
their timid mode of coping
with situations (Wolpe, 1958;
Wolpe & Lazarus, 1966). A variety
of
assertiveness
training programs have been
developed specifically for individuals
seeking to overcome
destructive
passivity. But assertiveness
training has also been
used in treating sexual
problems, depression,
and
marital conflicts. It is important to note that
cognitive self-statements (for example,
"I was thinking that
I
am perfectly free to say no")
may enhance the effects of
assertiveness training. Infact many
procedures
can
be used to increase assertiveness.
Behavior rehearsal is perhaps
the most obvious
one.
Lack
of assertiveness may stem from a
variety of sources. In some
cases, the cause may be a simple
lack of
information,
in which case the treatment
might center largely on
information giving. In other instances,
a
kind
of anticipatory anxiety may prevent
persons from behaving assertively. In
such cases, the
treatment
may
involve
desensitization.
Yet other individuals may have
unrealistic (negative) expectations about
what
will
ensue if they become
assertive. Some clinicians
would deal with such
expectations through
interpretation
or rational-emotive techniques.
Similar
techniques might be applied to
patients who feel that
assertiveness is wrong. Finally,
there are
patients
whose lack of assertiveness
involves a behavioral deficit
they do not know how to
behave
assertively,
for such patients, behavior
rehearsal, modeling, and related
procedures would be
used.
Assertiveness
training is not the same as
trying to teach people to be aggressive.
It is really a method of
training
people to express how they
feel without trampling on
the rights of other
in the
process (Wolpe &
Lazarus,
1966). Take the spectator at a basketball
game that cannot see because
the person in front
constantly
jumps up. To react by saying "If you
don't sit down, I'm going to
knock you down" is
aggressive.
But
saying "Please, I wish you
would sit down; I just can't
see anything" is an assertive
response. Indeed,
assertiveness
training has been useful in
teaching overly aggressive
persons gentler and more
effective ways
of
meeting their needs.
Contingency
Management:
A
variety of Skinnerian or operant
techniques are all referred to as
contingency
management procedures.
They
share the common goal of
controlling behavior by manipulating its
consequences.
Techniques:
Contingency
management can take many forms, of
which the following are just
a few examples.
1.
Shaping:
A
desired-behavior is developed by first rewarding
any behavior that approximates
it.
Gradually,
through selective reinforcement of
behavior more and more
closely resembling the
desired
behavior,
the final behavior is shaped.
This technique is sometimes called
successive
approximation.
2.
Time
Out: Undesirable
behavior is extinguished by removing the
person temporarily from a
situation in
which
that behavior is reinforced.
A child who disrupts the
class is removed so that the disruptive
behavior
cannot
be reinforced by the attention of
others.
3.
Contingency
Contracting: A formal
agreement or contract is-struck
between therapist and
patient,
specifying
the consequences of certain behaviors on
the part of both.
249
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
4.
"Grandma's
Rule'':
The
basic idea is akin to Grandma's
exhortation, "First you
work, then you play!" It
means that desired
activity
is reinforced by allowing the individual
the privilege of engaging in a more
attractive behavior. For
example,
the child is allowed to play
bail after the music lesson
is completed. This method is
sometimes
referred
to as the Premack
principle (Premark,
1959).
Token
Economies:
The
operant approach is most
commonly used in environments in which a
therapist or other institutional
staff
can exert significant control
over the- reinforcement
contingencies relative to patient
behavior. The
principles
of operant conditioning are especially
apparent in token
economy programs that
are designed to
modify
the behavior of institutionalized
populations; such as those with mental
retardation-or chronic
mental
illness (Kazdin, 1977; Liberman, 1972).
Such programs can make an
institution a more livable
place
that
ultimately is more conducive to therapeutic
gains. Many of the social
skills that are "shaped" will
also
facilitate
a smoother transition to a non
institutional setting.
In
establishing a token economy, there are
three major considerations (Krasner,
1971).
First,
there must be a clear and
careful specification of the desirable behaviors that
will be reinforced.
Second,
a clearly defined reinforcer
for medium of exchange-for example, colored poker
chips,
cards,
or
coins)
must be decided upon.
Third,
backup reinforcers are established.
These may be special
privileges or other things desired by
the
patient.
Thus, two tokens, each
worth 10 points, might be exchanged
for permission to watch TV n
extra
hour,
or one token worth 5 points
might be exchanged for a small
piece of candy. It goes without
saying
that
a token economy also
requires a fairly elaborate
system of record keeping and
a staff that is very
observant
and committed to the importance of the
program.
Token
economies are used to promote
desired behavior through the
control of reinforcements. Whether
the
desired
behavior is increased neatness,
greater social participation, or
improved job performance,
the
probability
of its occurrence can be enhanced by
the award of tokens of
varying value. But why
use tokens
at
all? Why not reinforce proper
bed making directly? The
reason is essentially that the effect of
rein-
forcement
is greater if the reinforcement occurs
immediately after the behavior
occurs. If the reward of
attending
a movie occurs ten hours
after a patient sweeps out
his or her room, it is not
likely to be nearly so
effective
as a token given immediately.
That token will come to
signify reward and will
assume much of the
effectiveness
of the backup reward for which it may be
exchanged.
250
Table of Contents:
|
|||||