ZeePedia

METHODS OF IMPROVING INTERPRETATION AND JUDGMENT

<< THE PROCESS AND ACCURACY OF CLINICAL JUDGEMENT:Comparison Studies
PSYCHOLOGICAL INTERVENTIONS AND THEIR GOALS:THE EXPERT ROLE >>
img
Clinical Psychology­ (PSY401)
VU
LESSON 24
METHODS OF IMPROVING INTERPRETATION AND JUDGMENT
There are variety of factors that can reduce the efficiency and validity of clinical predictions and
interpretation. One cannot presume to lay down a series of prescriptions that will lead inevitably to perfect
performance. Let us, however, call attention to several factors that are important to keep in mind as one
move from data to interpretation to prediction. Although the performance of clinicians has not been
good, there are ways of making improvements (Faust, 1986; Garb, 1998).
INFORMATION PROCESSING
As clinicians process assessment information, they are often bombarded with tremendous amounts of
data. In many instances, this information can be difficult to integrate because of its volume and complexity.
Clinicians must guard against the tendency to oversimplify. It is easy for them to overreact to a few
"eye-catching" bits o f information and to ignore other data that do not fit_ i n t o the picture they are
trying to paint. Whether the pressure comes from an overload of information or from a need to be
consistent in inferences about the patient, clinicians must be able to tolerate the ambiguity and com-
plexity that arise from patients who are inherently complex.
THE READING-IN SYNDROME
Clinicians sometimes tend to over interpret. They often inject meaning into remarks and actions that are
best regarded as less than deeply meaningful. Because clinicians are set to make such observations, they
can easily react to minimal cues as evidence of psychopathology. What is really amazing is that the
world gets along with 'so many "sick" people out there. It is so easy to emphasize the negative rather
than the positive that clinicians can readily make dire predictions or interpretation` that fail to take
the person's assets into account. Garb (1998) points out that clinicians who do evaluate clients'
strengths and assets in addition to assessing pathology and dysfunction are less likely to pronounce
clients as maladjusted or impaired.
VALIDATION AND RECORDS
Too often, clinicians make interpretations or predictions without following them up. if clinicians fail to
record interpretations and predictions, it becomes too easy to remember only the correct ones. Taking
pains to compare the clinician's view with that of professional colleagues, relatives, or others who know
the patient can also help to refine interpretive skills.
VAGUE REPORTS, CONCEPTS, AND CRITERIA
One of the most pervasive obstacles to valid clinical judgment is the tendency to use vague concepts and
poorly defined criteria. This process; of course culminates in psychological reports that are equally vague.
Under these conditions, it can be very difficult to determine whether clinicians' predictions and
judgments were correct (which may be why some of them use such shadowy terminology!). To combat
this problem, Garb (1998) recommends that clinicians use structured rating scales, objective personality
tests, and behavioral assessment methods to form their clinical judgment and predictions.
THE EFFECTS OF PREDICTION
Sometimes predictions turn out to be in error not because they were based on faulty inferences but
because the predictions themselves influenced the behavioral situation. For example, a prediction that a
patient would have difficulty adjusting at home after release from the hospital may have been correct.
However, the Patient's relatives may have accepted the prediction as a challenge and therefore provided
187
img
Clinical Psychology­ (PSY401)
VU
an environment that was more conducive to the patient's a d j u s t m e n t than it would have been in the
absence of the prediction Thus; the very act of having made a judgment may serve to alter the clinician's
own behavior or that of others.
PREDICTION TO UNKNOWN SITUATIONS
Clinical inferences and predictions are likely to be in error when clinicians are not clear about the
situations to which they are predicting. Inferring aggression from the TAT is one thing; relating it to
specific situations is another. Furthermore, no matter how careful and correct clinicians are, an
extraneous event can negate an otherwise perfectly valid prediction. Take the following example from
the OSS assessment program:
One high-ranking OSS officer while operating abroad, received a letter from a friend of
his in America informing him that his-wife had run off with the local garage man,
leaving no message or address. As a result the officer's morale, which had formerly been
high, dropped to zero. The assessment staff could predict that a small percentage of men
would have to cope with a profoundly depressing or disquieting event of this sort, but,
again, it was not possible to guess which of the assesses would be thus afflicted.
Co mmon sense should suggest that to accurately predict a person's behavior, the clinician must
consider the environment in which that behavior will take place this is also a tenet of behavioral
assessment. However, clinicians are frequently asked to make predictions based on only imprecise and
vague information regarding the situation in which their patient will be living or working.
In a hospital setting, a clinician may be requested to provide a prerelease workup on a given psychiatric
patient. But the information available to the clinician will too often cover only general background,
with supplementary descriptions of individual differences. Investigators such as Chase (1975),
Ekehammar (1974), Megargee (1970), Mischel (1968), and Moos (1975) all agree that such data are
subject to a ceiling effect that will allow correlations of no better than .30 to .40 between the data and
subsequent behavior. To say the least, correlation of that magnitude leaves a great deal to be desired.
Therefore, personality data alone are likely to be insufficient in many prediction situations.
FALLACIOUS PREDICTION PRINCIPLES
In some instances, intuitive predictions can lead clinicians into error because they ignore the logic of
statistical prediction. Intuitive predictions often ignore base rates, fail to consider regression effects, and
assume that highly correlated predictors will yield higher validity (Garb, 1998; Kahneman & Tversky,
1973). For example, suppose that a clinician is assessing a patient by collecting samples of behavior in a
variety of situations. Even though observations reveal an extremely aggressive person, the clinician
should not be surprised to learn that eventually the person behaves in a non aggressive fashion.
R e g r e s s i o n c o n c ep t s sh o u l d l e ad o n e t o e x p e c t that exceptionally tall parents will have a
shorter child that brilliant students sometimes do poorly, and so on.
In addition, clinicians' own confidence can sometimes be misleading. For example, Kahneman and
Tversky (1973) showed that individuals are more confident when they are predicting from correlated tests.
More specifically, although clinicians are often more confident of their inferences when they stem from
a combination of the Rorschach, the TAT, and the MMPI rather than from a single test, tit. Golden (1964) could
find no evidence to support this confidence. The reliability and validity of clinical interpretations did not
increase as a function of increasing amounts of test data. One should always seek to corroborate one's
inferences, but it would be a mistake to believe that the validity of inferences is inevitably correlated with
the size of the test battery.
188
img
Clinical Psychology­ (PSY401)
VU
THE INFLUENCE OF STEREOTYPED BELIEFS
Sometimes clinicians seem to interpret data in terms of stereotyped beliefs (Chapman & Chapman,
1967). For example, Golding and Rorer (1972) found that certain clinicians believed that anal responses
on the Rorschach indicated homosexuality; and they w-ere extremely resistant to changing their
preconceptions even in the face of intensive training to the contrary. Such research is a reminder that
clinicians must constantly be on guard against any tendency to believe that certain diagnostic signs
are inevitably valid indicators of certain characteristics.
Another example comes from a survey of the effects of clients' socioeconomic status on clinicians'
judgments (Sutton & Kessler, 1986). A sample of 242 respondents read case histories identical in all
respects except that the client was placed in different socioeconomic classes. When t h e cl i en t was
described as an unemp l oy ed welfare recipient with a seventh-grade education, clinicians predicted
a poorer prognosis and were less likely to recommend insight therapy.
"WHY I DO NOT ATTEND CASE CONFERENCES"
In an engaging paper, Meehl (1977) lists a variety of reasons why he gave up attending case conferences.
He catalogs a number of fallacies that often surface at such meetings. Most of them are entirely
relevant to the interpretive process generally. The following synopsis of a few of Meehl's examples
provides something of their general flavor:
· Sick-sick fallacy: the tendency to perceive people very unlike ourselves as being sick. There is a
tendency to interpret behavior very unlike our own as maladjusted, and it is easier to see pathology
in such clients.
· Me-too fallacy: denying the diagnostic significance of an event in the patient's life because it has
also happened to us. Some of us are narcissistic or defensive enough to believe we are paragons of
mental health. Therefore, the more our patients are like ourselves; the less likely we are to detect
problems.
· Uncle George's pancakes fallacy: "There is nothing wrong with that; my Uncle George did not
like to throw away leftover pancakes either." This is perhaps an extension of the previous fallacy.
Things that we do (and by extension, things that those close to us do) could not be maladjusted;
therefore, those like us cannot be maladjusted either.
· Multiple Napoleons fallacy: There was only one Napoleon, despite how strongly a psychotic
patient may feel that he or she is also Napoleon. An objection to interpreting such a patient's belief as
pathological is buttressed by the remark, "Well, it may not be real to us, but it's real to him (or her)!"
Further, "Everything is real to the person doing the perceiving. In fact, our percepts are our reality." If
this argument were invoked consistently, nothing could possibly be pathological. Even the patient with
paranoid schizophrenia who believes aliens are living in his nasal passages would be normal since; after
all, this is reality for him.
· Understanding it makes it normal fallacy: the idea that understanding a patient's beliefs or
behaviors strips them of their significance. This trap is very easy for clinicians to fall into. Even the
most deviant and curious behavior can somehow begin to seem acceptable once we convince
ourselves that we know the reasons for its occurrence. This may not be unlike the reasoning of
those who excuse the criminal's behavior because they understand the motives and poor child-
hood experiences involved.
189
img
Clinical Psychology­ (PSY401)
VU
CONCLUSION
There are number of recommendations to improve the reliability and validity of clinical judgment.
1) Consider all available information and do not ignore inconsistent data.
2) Consider clients' or patients' strengths and assets as well as pathology and dysfunction.
3) Documents all predictions, try to evaluate their accuracy, and use this information as feedback.
4) Use only structured interviews, structured ratting scales, objective personality tests, and
behavioral assessment methods to gather data.
5) Consider the client's situation and environment before making predictions.
6) Consider base rates and regression effects.
7) Do not let one's level of confidence influence prediction.
8) Be ware of and guard against stereotyped beliefs and illusory correlation.
THE CLINICAL REPORT WRITING: AN EFFECTIVE CLINICAL REPORT
COMMUNICATION: THE CLINICAL REPORT
After the clinician has completed the interview, administered the tests, and read the case history. The
tests have been scored, and hypotheses and impressions have been developed. The time has come to
write the report. This is the communication phase of the assessment process.
Appelbaum (1970) has characterized the role of the assessor as sociologist, politician, diplomat, group
dynamicist, salesperson, artist, and yes, even psychologist. As a sociologist, the assessor must assay the
local mores to aid in the acceptance of the report and to direct the report to those most likely to
implement it. In some instances, this may mean interacting directly with hospital personnel to
convince them of the validity of the report and to encourage them to act on it. These interactions may
involve ward attendants, nurses, psychiatrists, and others. Such persuasion may at times seem more
suitable for a politician or a diplomat than for a clinician.
One should not accept the role of clinical huckster. However, there are certainly times when reports
will have to serve the function of convincing reluctant others. Not everyone is willing to regard the
clinician as a purveyor of wisdom and unadulterated truth. Ideally, of course, the evidence for
clinicians' conclusions and the tightness of their arguments will be reasons enough for accepting their
descriptions and recommendations.
There is no single "best format" for a report. The nature of the referral, the audience to which the
report is directed, the kinds of assessment procedures used, and the theoretical persuasion of the clinician
are just a few of the considerations that may affect the presentation of a clinical report. What one says to
a psychiatrist is likely to be couched in language different from that directed to a school official. The
feedback provided to the parents of a mentally retarded child must be presented differently from the
190
img
Clinical Psychology­ (PSY401)
VU
feedback given to a professional colleague. Given below is a sample outline of a psychological test report
(Beutler, 1995).
1. Identifying question
a. Name of patient
b. Sex
c. Age
d. Ethnicity / Class
e. Date of evaluation
f.
Referring clinician
2. Referral question
3. Assessment procedures
4. Background
a. Information relevant to clarifying the referral question.
b. A statement of the probable reliabailty/validty of conclusion.
5. Summary of impressions and findings
a. Cognitive level
Patient's intellectual and cognitive functioning (ideation, intelligence, memory,
perception)
Degree (amount of impairment) compared to premorbid level.
Probable cause of impairment.
b. Affective and mood levels
Mood, affect at present---compare with premorbid levels.
Degree of disturbance (mild, moderate, sever).
Chronic versus acute nature of disturbance.
Lability--how well can the person modulate, control affects with his/her
cognitive resources?
c. Interpersonal-intrapersonal level
Primary interpersonal and interpersonal conflicts and their significance.
Interpersonal and intrapersonal coping strategies.
Formulation of personality.
6. Diagnostic-interpersonal levels
a. Series of impressions about cognitive and affective functioning, or
b. The most probable diagnosis.
7. Recommendations
a. Assessment of risk, need for confinement, medication.
b. Duration, modality, frequency of treatment.
191
img
Clinical Psychology­ (PSY401)
VU
THE REFERRAL SOURCE
The major responsibility of the report is to address the referral question. The test report should carefully
and explicitly answer the questions that prompted the assessment in the first place. If the referral
questions cannot be answered or if they are somehow inappropriate, this should be stated in the report
and the reasons given for this judgment. In some (perhaps most) instances, contradictions will be
inherent in the assessment data. Although the clinician must make every effort to resolve such
contradictions and present a unified view of the patient, there are instances in which such resolution
is not possible. In those instances, the contradictions should be described. Distortion in the service of
consistency is not a desirable alternative.
There are often secondary readers of clinical reports for example, although the primary report may
be sent to the referring person (a psychiatrist, another clinician, or an agency), a secondary reader
may be an agency administrator, ad program evaluator, or a research psychologist. In specific
circumstances it may be necessary or even desirable to prepare a special report for such people. In any
event, a clinical report does not always serve an exclusively clinical or direct helping function. It can also
be useful in assisting an agency to evaluate the effect of its programs. It can likewise be useful from the
standpoint of psychological research. Information in clinical reports can often be helpful in validating tests
or the interpretations and predictions made from tests. Such data can sometimes provide a baseline against
which to compare subsequent change in the patient as a function of various forms of intervention.
AIDS TO COMMUNICATION
The function of a report is communication. The following are some suggestions for enhancing that
function.
Language
One should not resort to jargon or to a boring and detailed test-by-test account of patient responses. Again,
it is important to recall the nature of the referral source. In general, it is probably best to write in a style
and language that can be understood by the intelligent layperson. Of course, what is jargon or excessively
technical is partly in the eye of the beholder. A considerable amount of technical language can be
tolerated in a report sent to professional colleagues whom one knows. On the other hand, technical jargon
has no place in a report that is going to a parent. The terms interest scatter and Erlebnistypus may be
all right for another clinician, but they should not appear in a report sent to a junior high school counselor.
Individualized Reports
We know the importance of avoiding the Barnum effect (a term applied in case where statements that appears to
be valid self-descriptions in actually characterize almost everybody).The distinctive (be it current
characteristics, development, or learning history) is preferred over the general. To say "Jack is insecure"
hardly distinguishes him from 90% of all psychotherapy patients. To say that Jack's insecurity stems from
a history of living with several different relatives as a child and that it will become particularly acute
Whenever he must make a decision that will make him away (even temporally) from the home-is
considerably more meaningful. In this case, a general characteristic has been distinctly qualified by both
antecedent and subsequent conditions.
The Level of Detail
The question often arises as to how detailed a report ought to be. Again, the answer depends largely
on the audience. In general, however, it seems desirable to include a mix of abstract generalities, specific
behavioral illustrations, and some testing detail. For example, in reporting depressive tendencies, a few
illustrations of the test responses that led to the inference would be in order. A few of the relevant
behavioral observations that were made during testing could also be quite helpful. A certain amount of
192
img
Clinical Psychology­ (PSY401)
VU
detail can give readers the feeling that they can evaluate the clinician's conclusions and interpretations.
The exclusive use of abstract generalities places the reader at the mercy of the author's inferential
processes.
CONCLUSION
The clinical report serves as the major form of communication to convey the findings from a clinician's
assessment and evaluation. The report should address the referral questions, using language that is
tailored to the person or persons who will be reading the report. Finally, the report should contain
information that is detailed and specific to the client and should avoid vague, Barnum like statements.
193
Table of Contents:
  1. MENTAL HEALTH TODAY: A QUICK LOOK OF THE PICTURE:PARA-PROFESSIONALS
  2. THE SKILLS & ACTIVITIES OF A CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGIST:THE INTERNSHIP
  3. HOW A CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGIST THINKS:Brian’s Case; an example, PREDICTION
  4. HISTORICAL OVERVIEW OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY:THE GREEK PERIOD
  5. HISTORY OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY:Research, Assessment, CONCLUSION
  6. HOW CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGISTS BECAME INVOLVED IN TREATMENT
  7. MODELS OF TRAINING IN CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY:PROFESSIONAL SCHOOLS
  8. CURRENT ISSUES IN CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY:CERTIFICATION, LICENSING
  9. ETHICAL STANDARDS FOR CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGISTS:PREAMBLE
  10. THE ROLE OF RESEARCH IN CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY:LIMITATION
  11. THE RESEARCH PROCESS:GENERATING HYPOTHESES, RESEARCH METHODS
  12. THE CONCEPT OF ABNORMAL BEHAVIOR & MENTAL ILLNESS
  13. CAUSES OF MENTAL ILLNESOVERVIEW OF ETIOLOGY:PANDAS
  14. THE PROCESS OF DIAGNOSIS:ADVANTAGES OF DIAGNOSIS, DESCRIPTION
  15. THE CONCEPT OF PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT IN CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY
  16. THE CLINICAL INTERVIEW:The intake / admission interview, Structured interview
  17. THE ASSESSMENT OF INTELLIGENCE:RELIABILTY AND VALIDITY, CATTELL’S THEORY
  18. INTELLIGENCE TESTS:PURPOSE, COMMON PROCEDURES, PURPOSE
  19. THE USE AND ABUSE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING:PERSONALITY
  20. THE PROJECTIVE PERSONALITY TESTS:THE RORSCHACH
  21. THE OBSERVATIONAL ASSESSMENT AND ITS TYPES:Home Observation
  22. THE BEHAVIORAL ASSESSMENT THROUGH INTERVIEWS, INVENTORIES AND CHECK LISTS
  23. THE PROCESS AND ACCURACY OF CLINICAL JUDGEMENT:Comparison Studies
  24. METHODS OF IMPROVING INTERPRETATION AND JUDGMENT
  25. PSYCHOLOGICAL INTERVENTIONS AND THEIR GOALS:THE EXPERT ROLE
  26. IMPORTANCE OF PSYCHOTHERAPY:ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS
  27. COURSE OF NEW CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS:IMPLEMENTING TREATMENT
  28. NATURE OF SPECIFIC THERAPEUTIC VARIABLES:CLIENT’S MOTIVATION
  29. THE BEGINNING OF PSYCHOANALYSIS:THE CASE OF ANNA, THE INSTINCTS
  30. PSYCHOANALYTIC ALTERNATIVES:EGO ANALYSIS, CURATIVE FACTORS
  31. CLIENT CENTERED THERAPY:PURPOSE, BACKGROUND, PROCESS
  32. GESTALT THERAPY METHODS AND PROCEDURES:SELF-DIALOGUE
  33. ORIGINS AND TRADITIONAL TECHNIQUES OF BEHAVIOR THERAPY
  34. COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY:MODELING, RATIONAL RESTRUCTURING
  35. GROUP THERAPY: METHODS AND PROCEDURES:CURATIVE FACTORS
  36. FAMILY AND COUPLES THERAPY:POSSIBLE RISKS
  37. INTRODUCTION AND HISTORY OF COMMUNITY PSYCHOLOGY:THE ENVIRONMENT
  38. METHODS OF INTERVENTION AND CHANGE IN COMMUNITY PSYCHOLOGY
  39. INTRODUCTION AND HISTORY OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY
  40. APPLICATIONS OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY:OBESITY, HEALTH CARE TRENDS
  41. NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PERSPECTIVES AND HISTORY:STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION
  42. METHODS OF NEUROLOGICAL ASSESSMENT:Level Of Performance, Pattern Analysis
  43. FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY:Qualification, Testifying, Cross Examination, Criminal Cases
  44. PEDIATRIC AND CHILD PSYCHOLOGY: HISTORY AND PERSPECTIVE
  45. INTERVENTIONS & TRAINING IN PEDIATRIC AND CLINICAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY