|
|||||
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
LESSON
24
METHODS OF
IMPROVING INTERPRETATION AND
JUDGMENT
There
are variety of factors that
can reduce the efficiency and
validity of clinical predictions
and
interpretation.
One cannot presume to lay
down a series of prescriptions
that will lead inevitably to
perfect
performance.
Let us, however, call
attention to several factors that
are important to keep in mind as
one
move
from data to interpretation to
prediction. Although the performance of
clinicians has not
been
good,
there are ways of making improvements
(Faust, 1986; Garb, 1998).
INFORMATION
PROCESSING
As
clinicians process assessment
information, they are often
bombarded with tremendous
amounts of
data.
In many instances, this information
can be difficult to integrate
because of its volume and
complexity.
Clinicians
must guard against the tendency to
oversimplify. It is easy for
them to overreact to a
few
"eye-catching"
bits o f information and to ignore other
data that do not fit_ i
n t o the picture they
are
trying
to paint. Whether the
pressure comes from an
overload of information or from a
need to be
consistent
in inferences about the
patient, clinicians must be able to
tolerate the ambiguity and
com-
plexity
that arise from patients
who are inherently
complex.
THE
READING-IN SYNDROME
Clinicians
sometimes tend to over
interpret. They often inject
meaning into remarks and
actions that are
best
regarded as less than deeply
meaningful. Because clinicians are
set to make such
observations, they
can
easily react to minimal cues
as evidence of psychopathology. What is
really amazing is that
the
world
gets along with 'so many
"sick" people out there. It is so
easy to emphasize the negative
rather
than
the positive that clinicians
can readily make dire
predictions or interpretation` that
fail to take
the
person's assets into account. Garb
(1998) points out that
clinicians who do evaluate
clients'
strengths
and assets in addition to
assessing pathology and
dysfunction are less likely
to pronounce
clients
as maladjusted or impaired.
VALIDATION
AND RECORDS
Too
often, clinicians make interpretations or predictions
without following them up.
if clinicians
fail to
record
interpretations and predictions, it
becomes too easy to remember
only the correct ones.
Taking
pains
to compare the clinician's view
with that of professional colleagues,
relatives, or others who
know
the
patient can also help to
refine interpretive
skills.
VAGUE
REPORTS, CONCEPTS, AND
CRITERIA
One
of the most pervasive
obstacles to valid clinical
judgment is the tendency to use vague
concepts and
poorly
defined criteria. This
process;
of
course culminates in psychological
reports that are equally
vague.
Under
these conditions, it can be
very difficult to determine
whether clinicians' predictions
and
judgments
were correct (which may be
why some of them use such
shadowy terminology!). To
combat
this
problem, Garb (1998)
recommends that clinicians
use structured rating
scales, objective personality
tests,
and behavioral assessment
methods to form their
clinical judgment and
predictions.
THE
EFFECTS OF PREDICTION
Sometimes
predictions turn out to be in
error not because they
were based on faulty
inferences but
because
the predictions themselves
influenced the behavioral situation.
For example, a prediction
that a
patient
would have difficulty adjusting at home
after release from the hospital may have
been correct.
However,
the Patient's relatives may have
accepted the prediction as a
challenge and therefore
provided
187
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
an
environment that was more
conducive to the patient's a d j u s t m e n t
than it would have been in
the
absence
of the prediction Thus; the
very act of having made a
judgment may serve to alter the
clinician's
own
behavior or that of
others.
PREDICTION
TO UNKNOWN SITUATIONS
Clinical
inferences and predictions are
likely to be in error when
clinicians are not clear
about the
situations
to which they are
predicting. Inferring aggression
from the TAT is one thing;
relating it to
specific
situations is another. Furthermore, no
matter how careful and
correct clinicians are,
an
extraneous
event can negate an otherwise
perfectly valid prediction.
Take the following example
from
the
OSS assessment program:
One
high-ranking OSS officer while
operating abroad, received a letter from
a friend of
his
in America informing him
that his-wife had run
off with the local garage
man,
leaving
no message or address. As a result
the officer's morale, which had formerly
been
high,
dropped to zero. The assessment
staff could predict that a small
percentage of men
would
have to cope with a
profoundly depressing or disquieting
event of this sort,
but,
again,
it was not possible to guess
which of the assesses would
be thus afflicted.
Co
mmon sense should suggest
that to accurately predict a
person's behavior, the
clinician must
consider
the environment in which
that behavior will take
place this is also a tenet of
behavioral
assessment.
However, clinicians are
frequently asked to make
predictions based on only
imprecise and
vague
information regarding the
situation in which their patient
will be living or
working.
In
a hospital setting, a clinician may be
requested to provide a prerelease workup
on a given psychiatric
patient.
But the information available to
the clinician will too often
cover only general
background,
with
supplementary descriptions of individual differences.
Investigators such as Chase
(1975),
Ekehammar
(1974), Megargee (1970),
Mischel (1968), and Moos
(1975) all agree that
such data are
subject
to a ceiling effect that
will allow correlations of no better than
.30 to .40 between the data
and
subsequent
behavior. To say the least,
correlation of that magnitude leaves a
great deal to be
desired.
Therefore,
personality data alone are
likely to be insufficient in many
prediction situations.
FALLACIOUS
PREDICTION PRINCIPLES
In
some instances, intuitive predictions
can lead clinicians into
error because they ignore
the logic of
statistical
prediction. Intuitive predictions
often ignore base rates,
fail to consider regression
effects, and
assume
that highly correlated predictors will
yield higher validity (Garb,
1998; Kahneman & Tversky,
1973).
For example, suppose that a
clinician is assessing a patient by
collecting samples of behavior in
a
variety
of situations. Even though observations
reveal an extremely aggressive
person, the clinician
should
not be surprised
to
learn that eventually the
person behaves in a non aggressive
fashion.
R
e g r e s s i o n c o n c ep t s sh o u l d l e ad o n e t o e x p e c t
that exceptionally tall parents will
have a
shorter
child that brilliant
students sometimes do poorly,
and so on.
In
addition, clinicians' own
confidence can sometimes be misleading.
For example, Kahneman
and
Tversky
(1973) showed that
individuals are more
confident when they are
predicting from correlated
tests.
More
specifically, although clinicians
are often more confident of
their inferences when they
stem from
a
combination of the Rorschach,
the TAT, and the MMPI
rather than from a single
test, tit. Golden (1964)
could
find
no evidence to support this confidence.
The reliability and validity
of clinical interpretations did
not
increase
as a function of increasing amounts of
test data. One should always
seek to corroborate
one's
inferences,
but it would be a mistake to
believe that the validity of inferences
is inevitably correlated with
the
size of the test
battery.
188
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
THE
INFLUENCE OF STEREOTYPED
BELIEFS
Sometimes
clinicians seem to interpret
data in terms of stereotyped
beliefs (Chapman &
Chapman,
1967).
For example, Golding and
Rorer (1972) found that certain
clinicians believed
that anal responses
on
the Rorschach indicated homosexuality;
and they w-ere
extremely resistant to changing
their
preconceptions
even in the face of intensive training to
the contrary. Such research
is a reminder that
clinicians
must constantly be on guard against
any tendency to believe that
certain diagnostic
signs
are
inevitably valid indicators of certain
characteristics.
Another
example comes from a survey of the effects of
clients' socioeconomic status on
clinicians'
judgments
(Sutton & Kessler, 1986). A sample of
242 respondents read case
histories identical in
all
respects
except that the client was
placed in different socioeconomic
classes. When t h e cl i en t
was
described
as an unemp l oy ed welfare recipient
with a seventh-grade education,
clinicians predicted
a
poorer prognosis and were less
likely to recommend insight
therapy.
"WHY
I DO NOT ATTEND CASE CONFERENCES"
In
an engaging paper, Meehl
(1977) lists a variety of
reasons why he gave up
attending case
conferences.
He
catalogs a number of fallacies that
often surface at such meetings.
Most of them are
entirely
relevant
to the interpretive process
generally. The following
synopsis of a few of Meehl's
examples
provides
something of their general
flavor:
·
Sick-sick
fallacy:
the
tendency to perceive people
very unlike ourselves as being
sick. There is a
tendency
to interpret behavior very
unlike our own as maladjusted,
and it is easier to see
pathology
in
such clients.
·
Me-too
fallacy:
denying
the diagnostic significance of an event in the
patient's life because it
has
also
happened to us. Some of us are
narcissistic or defensive enough to
believe we are paragons
of
mental
health. Therefore, the more
our patients are like
ourselves; the less likely we
are to detect
problems.
·
Uncle
George's pancakes
fallacy:
"There
is nothing wrong with that;
my Uncle George did
not
like
to throw away leftover
pancakes either." This is perhaps an
extension of the previous
fallacy.
Things
that we do (and by extension,
things that those close to us do)
could not be
maladjusted;
therefore,
those like us cannot be maladjusted
either.
·
Multiple
Napoleons fallacy:
There
was only one Napoleon,
despite how strongly a
psychotic
patient
may feel that he or she is
also Napoleon. An objection to
interpreting such a patient's
belief as
pathological
is buttressed by the remark,
"Well, it may not be real to
us, but it's real to him (or
her)!"
Further,
"Everything is real to the person doing
the perceiving. In fact, our percepts
are our reality." If
this
argument were invoked consistently,
nothing could possibly be pathological. Even the
patient with
paranoid
schizophrenia who believes aliens
are living in his nasal
passages would be normal since;
after
all,
this is reality for
him.
·
Understanding
it makes it normal fallacy:
the idea
that understanding a patient's beliefs
or
behaviors
strips them of their significance.
This trap is very easy
for clinicians to fall into.
Even the
most
deviant and curious behavior
can somehow begin to seem
acceptable once we convince
ourselves
that we know the reasons
for its occurrence. This may
not be unlike the reasoning
of
those
who excuse the criminal's
behavior because they
understand the motives and
poor child-
hood
experiences involved.
189
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
CONCLUSION
There
are number of recommendations to improve the
reliability and validity of clinical
judgment.
1)
Consider all available information
and do not ignore inconsistent
data.
2)
Consider clients' or patients' strengths
and assets as well as
pathology and
dysfunction.
3)
Documents all predictions,
try to evaluate their accuracy,
and use this information as
feedback.
4)
Use only structured interviews,
structured ratting scales, objective
personality tests,
and
behavioral
assessment methods to gather
data.
5)
Consider the client's situation and
environment before making
predictions.
6)
Consider base rates and
regression effects.
7)
Do not let one's level of
confidence influence
prediction.
8)
Be ware of and guard against stereotyped
beliefs and illusory
correlation.
THE
CLINICAL REPORT WRITING: AN
EFFECTIVE CLINICAL
REPORT
COMMUNICATION:
THE CLINICAL
REPORT
After
the clinician has completed the
interview, administered the tests, and
read the case history.
The
tests
have been scored, and hypotheses and impressions
have been developed. The
time has come to
write
the report. This is the
communication phase of the
assessment process.
Appelbaum
(1970) has characterized the
role of the assessor as sociologist,
politician, diplomat,
group
dynamicist,
salesperson, artist, and yes, even
psychologist. As a sociologist, the
assessor must assay
the
local
mores to aid in the
acceptance of the report and to
direct the report to those most likely
to
implement
it. In some instances, this
may mean interacting
directly with hospital
personnel to
convince
them of the validity of the report
and to encourage them to act
on it. These interactions
may
involve
ward attendants, nurses, psychiatrists,
and others. Such persuasion
may at times seem
more
suitable
for a politician or a diplomat
than for a clinician.
One
should not accept the
role of clinical huckster.
However, there are certainly
times when reports
will
have to serve the function of convincing
reluctant others. Not
everyone is willing to regard
the
clinician
as a purveyor of wisdom and
unadulterated truth. Ideally, of
course, the evidence
for
clinicians'
conclusions and the tightness of
their arguments will be
reasons enough for accepting
their
descriptions
and recommendations.
There
is no single "best format"
for a report. The nature of the
referral, the audience to which
the
report
is directed, the kinds of assessment
procedures used, and the
theoretical persuasion of the
clinician
are
just a few of the
considerations that may affect the
presentation of a clinical report. What
one says to
a
psychiatrist is likely to be couched in
language different from that directed to
a school official. The
feedback
provided to the parents of a mentally
retarded child must be presented differently
from the
190
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
feedback
given to a professional colleague.
Given below is a sample
outline of a psychological test
report
(Beutler,
1995).
1.
Identifying question
a.
Name of patient
b.
Sex
c.
Age
d.
Ethnicity / Class
e.
Date of evaluation
f.
Referring
clinician
2.
Referral question
3.
Assessment procedures
4.
Background
a.
Information relevant to clarifying
the referral
question.
b.
A statement of the probable
reliabailty/validty of conclusion.
5.
Summary of impressions and
findings
a.
Cognitive level
Patient's
intellectual and cognitive functioning
(ideation, intelligence,
memory,
perception)
Degree
(amount of impairment) compared to
premorbid level.
Probable
cause of impairment.
b.
Affective and mood
levels
Mood,
affect at present---compare with
premorbid levels.
Degree
of disturbance (mild, moderate,
sever).
Chronic
versus acute nature of
disturbance.
Lability--how
well can the person
modulate, control affects
with his/her
cognitive
resources?
c.
Interpersonal-intrapersonal level
Primary
interpersonal and interpersonal conflicts
and their significance.
Interpersonal
and intrapersonal coping
strategies.
Formulation
of personality.
6.
Diagnostic-interpersonal
levels
a.
Series of impressions about cognitive and
affective functioning, or
b.
The most probable
diagnosis.
7.
Recommendations
a.
Assessment of risk, need for
confinement, medication.
b.
Duration, modality, frequency of
treatment.
191
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
THE
REFERRAL SOURCE
The
major responsibility of the report is to
address the referral
question. The
test report should
carefully
and
explicitly answer the questions
that prompted the assessment in the
first place. If the
referral
questions
cannot be answered or if they
are somehow inappropriate, this
should be stated in the
report
and
the reasons given for
this judgment. In some
(perhaps most) instances, contradictions
will be
inherent
in the assessment data. Although
the clinician must make
every effort to resolve
such
contradictions
and present a unified view of
the patient, there are
instances in which such
resolution
is
not possible. In those instances, the
contradictions should be described.
Distortion in the service of
consistency
is not a desirable alternative.
There
are often secondary readers of
clinical reports for
example, although the
primary report may
be
sent to the referring person (a
psychiatrist, another clinician, or an
agency), a secondary reader
may
be an agency administrator, ad program
evaluator, or a research psychologist. In
specific
circumstances
it may be necessary or even
desirable to prepare a special
report for such people. In
any
event,
a clinical report does not
always serve an exclusively
clinical or direct helping function. It
can also
be
useful in assisting an agency to evaluate
the effect of its programs. It
can likewise be useful from
the
standpoint
of psychological research. Information in
clinical reports can often
be helpful in validating
tests
or
the interpretations and predictions made
from tests. Such data
can sometimes provide a baseline
against
which
to compare subsequent change in the
patient as a function of various forms of
intervention.
AIDS
TO COMMUNICATION
The
function of a report is communication.
The following are some
suggestions for enhancing
that
function.
Language
One
should not resort to jargon or to a
boring and detailed
test-by-test account of patient
responses. Again,
it
is important to recall the nature of the
referral source. In general, it is
probably best to write in a
style
and
language that can be understood by
the intelligent layperson. Of course,
what is jargon or excessively
technical
is partly in the eye of the beholder. A
considerable amount of technical language can
be
tolerated
in a report sent to professional
colleagues whom one knows.
On the other hand, technical
jargon
has
no place in a report that is going to a
parent. The terms interest
scatter and
Erlebnistypus
may
be
all
right for another clinician,
but they should not appear
in a report sent to a junior
high school
counselor.
Individualized
Reports
We
know the importance of
avoiding the Barnum effect
(a term applied in case where
statements that appears
to
be
valid self-descriptions in actually
characterize almost everybody).The
distinctive (be it
current
characteristics,
development, or learning history) is
preferred over the general. To say
"Jack is insecure"
hardly
distinguishes him from 90%
of all psychotherapy patients. To
say that Jack's insecurity stems
from
a
history of living with
several different relatives as a child
and that it will become
particularly acute
Whenever
he must make a decision that will
make him away (even
temporally) from the
home-is
considerably
more meaningful. In this
case, a general characteristic has been
distinctly qualified by
both
antecedent
and subsequent conditions.
The
Level of Detail
The
question often arises as to
how detailed a report ought
to be. Again, the answer
depends largely
on
the audience. In general, however, it
seems desirable to include a mix of
abstract generalities, specific
behavioral
illustrations, and some
testing detail. For example,
in reporting depressive tendencies, a
few
illustrations
of the test responses that
led to the inference would be in order. A
few of the relevant
behavioral
observations that were made
during testing could also be
quite helpful. A certain amount
of
192
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
detail
can give readers the
feeling that they can
evaluate the clinician's
conclusions and interpretations.
The
exclusive use of abstract
generalities places the reader at
the mercy of the author's
inferential
processes.
CONCLUSION
The
clinical report serves as the
major form of communication to
convey the findings from a
clinician's
assessment
and evaluation. The report
should address the referral questions,
using language that is
tailored
to the person or persons who
will be reading the report.
Finally, the report should
contain
information
that is detailed and specific to the
client and should avoid
vague, Barnum like
statements.
193
Table of Contents:
|
|||||