|
|||||
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
LESSON
23
THE
PROCESS AND ACCURACY OF CLINICAL
JUDGEMENT
As
scientific and objective as clinical psychology
has tried to become, it is
still virtually impossible
to
evaluate
its diagnostic and
assessment techniques 'apart from the
clinician involved.
Clinical
Judgment,"
is enough to suggest that
clinicians use inferential
processes that are often far
from
objective.
The process, accuracy, and communication
of clinical judgment are still
very often extremely
personalized
phenomena.
In
the discussion ahead we will examine
some of the means by which the
clinicians put together
assessment
data and arrive at
particular conclusion. We will also
discuss the accuracy of
clinical
judgment
and impressions.
PROCESS
AND ACCURACY
The
discussion of clinical judgment
will begin with its
basic element----Interpretation.
INTERPRETATION
It
is hard to disagree with L. H.
Levy's (1963) statement that
"Interpretation is the most
important single
activity
engaged in by the clinician".
Interpretation is an inferential process
(Nisbett & Ross,
1980)
that
takes where assessment
leaves off. The interviews have
been completed; the
psychological tests
have
been
administered. Now, what does it
all mean, and what
decisions are to be
made?
At
the very least, clinical
interpretation
or
judgment is a complex process. It
involves
1.
Stimuli----an MMPI-2 profile, an IQ
score, a gesture, a sound,
etc.
2.
It also involves the
clinician's response. "Is
this patient psychotic?" "Is
the patient's behavior
expressive
of a low expectancy for
success?" Or even "What is
the patient like?"
3.
It also involves the
characteristics of clinicians their
cognitive structures and
theoretical orientations.
4.
Finally, situational variables
enter into the process.
These can include everything
from the type and
range
of patients to the constraints that the demands of the
setting place on prediction$.
For
example,
a clinician in a university mental
health center may make a
range of judgments from
hospitalization
to psychotherapy to just dropping
out of school---whereas a clinician in a
prison
setting
may be limited to many fewer
options.
THE
THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
As
we know that clinical psychologists
strive to discover the etiology, or origins, of
psychological
problems
and to understand patients so
that they can be helped.
Clinical problems can be
conceptualized
in a variety of ways (for example,
psychodynamic, behavioral, and
cognitive). The kinds
of
interpretations made by a Freudian
are vastly different from t
h o s e m a d e by a behavioral
clinician.
Two
clinicians may each observe
t h a t a chiid persistently attempts to
sleep in his mother's bed.
For
the Freudians, this becomes
a sign of an unresolved Oedipus
complex. For the behaviorist,
the
interpretation
may be in terms of reinforcement.
Indeed,
one way in which clinicians
can evaluate interpretations is by
examining their consistency
with
the
theory from which they
are derived. The number of
interpretations that can be
made from a set of
observations,
interview responses, or test
data is both awesome and
bewildering. By adopting a
178
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
particular
theoretical perspective, clinicians can
evaluate interpretations and inferences
according to
their
theoretical consistency and can
also generate additional
hypotheses.
SIGNS,
SAMPLES AND CORRELATES
Patient
data can be viewed in
several ways.
1.
Samples: First, one
can view such data as
samples.
Observations,
test scores, test responses,
or
other
data are seen as samples of
a larger pool of information that
could be obtained outside
the
consulting
room. For example, when a
patient does poorly on the
Wechsler Memory
Scale,
this could be regarded as a
sample of nontest behavior (memory
problems).
2.
Signs:
A
second way in which patient
data can be interpreted is as
signs of some underlying
state,
condition, or determinant. Aside from
radical behaviorists, many clinicians will
seek to
infer
from observations of the
patient's behavior and test
responses a variety of
underlying
determinants.
For some clinicians, the
underlying determinant might be
anxiety, for others,
ego
strength;
and for still others,
expectancies. But in every case,
the observation is seen as
something that
signifies
underlying determinants. For example,
poor on a patient's Rorschach
responses is often
interpreted
as a sign of poor reality testing
(psychosis).
3.
Correlates:
A
third view of patient data
emphasizes their status as
correlates
of
other things. Once
the
anxious behavior, the flat
affect, or the inability to concentrate
have been noted in a
depressed
patient,
the clinician might predict an
associated decline in sexual activity, in
social relationships,
in
willingness to seek employment,
and so on. In effect, then,
assessment data can be
inter-
preted
to suggest behavioral, attitudinal or
emotional correlates.
LEVELS
OF INTERPRETATION
Whether
clinicians view clinical
data as samples, signs, or
correlates, they are making
inferences that will
enable
them to go from those
clinical data to recommendations, reports
or predictions. Sundberg, Tyler,
and
Taplin (1973) have described
three levels of inferences or
interpretations.
LEVEL
1 interpretation
generally involves little in the
way of inference and certainly
nothing in the way
of
a sign approach. From input to
output, there are practically no
intervening steps. For example, if
it
is
known on the basis of past
experience that students who
sit in the front row of a
class almost always get
A's
or
B's, then clinicians can go
directly from seat number to
grade prediction without any
necessity for
intervening
attributions of intelligence scores,
previous courses, and so on.
This simple yet efficient
ap-
proach
can dispense with high-level
clinicians who make exotic
inferences prior to their
predictions;
it
can be handled by technicians, computers,
or machines. Level I interpretations
can often be used
with
large
populations if the prime purpose is
screening and if predicting the
outcome for a specific
person is
relatively
unimportant. A college entrance exam is a
case in point. Here a single
test score may
predict
with
considerable accuracy the
academic performance of 1,000 freshmen.
Although that single score
may be
erroneous
as a predictor for student X, a
certain degree of error can
easily be tolerated if one is
interested-
primarily
in the number who is likely
to graduate.
LEVEL
II interpretations
involve two kinds'
inferences. The clinician
may observe a patient and
then
conclude
that the observe behavior generally
characterizes the patient. Sundberg etal
call this first kind
of
inference
descriptive generalization-----still
at the descriptive level.
Thus, for a patient who
fidgets,
smokes
cigarettes during the
interview, and stammers the
clinician may make a
descriptive generalization---
-interview
tension. If it turns out later that
the patient has trouble relaxing at
home, cannot sit through
the
meeting
at the office. And is very
worried about paying off
the mortage, the clinician
may go to a broader
descriptive
generalization. The second kind of
inference is a hypothetical construct
that suggests an inner
state
and
takes the clinician a bit
beyond descriptive generalaization.When clinicians
begin to make generalization
179
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
and
particularly, to impute inner
determinants to the patient, they are
moving directly to clinical
interpretation
as
it is often used.
LEVEL
III interpretations
take clinicians beyond level II primarily
by being more inclusive and
better
integrated.
at this level, they attempt to achieve a
consistent, broad understanding of
the `individual in
situation',
clinician will draw and
integrated picture of the patient's
developmental, social,
and
psychological
determinants that involves a
highly articulated theoretical system of
hypothesis and
deductions.
For example, a preponderance of `blood'
responses on the Rorschach
might be interpreted as a
sign
of underlying aggression that
may lead to future impulsive
outbursts or loss of control.
THEORY
AND INTERPRETATION
Currently,
clinicians may be assigned to three
very broad interpretive
classes.
BEHAVIORAL
CLINICIANS
First,
there are the behavioral
clinicians. The strict behaviorist avoids
making inferences about
underlying
states
and instead concentrates on
the
behavior
of the patient. The behavioral clinician
typically seeks
patient
data based on personal
observation or on direct reports
from the patient or
from the other observers.
These
data are regarded as
samples. Interpretation is Largely at
Level I and II, although
more recently some
behavioral
clinicians have begun to show an
interest in Level III
interpretation.
PSYCHOMETRIC
APPROACH
A
second group of clinicians
pride themselves on being
empirical and objective. In
particular, these
clinicians
are likely to use objective
tests to predict to relatively
specific criteria. For example, will
scores
from
tests A, B, and C predict success in
college, therapy outcome, or aggressive
out bursts? This
psychometric
approach to
interpretation, as we shall see a
bit later, is especially useful
when the criteria
being
predicted are crisp and well
articulated. In general, this approach
uses data as correlates
of
something
else-for example, a score at
the 95th percentile on test X may be
related to recidivism in
prisoners.
The psychometrically oriented clinician is
most concerned with
standardized tests and
their
norms,
regression equations, or actuarial tables, and
tends to employ_ Level
I and II interpretation.
PSYCHODYNAMIC
APPROACH
A
third group of clinicians is more
comfortable with a
p s y c h o d y n a m i c a p p r o a c h . This has
long been
a
popular orientation in clinical
psychology. Although current
clinicians often seem to opt
for a more
objective
behavioral or psychometric approach,
there is still more of the
psychodynamicist in many of
them
than they might like to
admit. The psychodynamic
approach strives to identify
inner states or
determinants.
Data from projective tests,
unstructured clinical interviews and
other sources are viewed
as
signs
of an underlying state. Interpretation
tends to itched at Level III. A broad,
often highly
impressionistic
picture of the patient is drawn,
although in many instances
subtle normative assertions
are
made.
QUANTITATIVE
VERSUS SUBJECTIVE
APPROACHES
Quietly
embedded in the preceding discussion
are two distinct approaches
to clinical judgment
and
interpretation.
First is the quantitative
or statistical approach, which
emphasizes objectivity and
is
presumably
free from fuzzy thinking.
Second is the subjective
or clinical approach, which
adherents
claim
is the only method to offer truly
useful interpretations and
predictions.
THE
QUANTITATIVE STATISTICAL
APPROACH
Perhaps
the simplest form of quantitative
prediction that clinicians
can use involves_ assigning
scores
180
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
to
the various characteristics of
their patients. This enables clinicians
to determine the
correlation
between
any two characteristics. For
example, suppose that after
several years of practice, a
clinician
begins
to suspect a direct relationship between
early termination of therapy
and patients' needs
for
independence.
The clinician might attempt to
verify this hypothesis by correlating
"need for
independence"
scores from a self-report
inventory with the length of
time that patients remain
in
therapy.
Should the correlation turn
out to be substantially above
.50, the clinician could
use need
for
independence scores to make
interpretations and predictions regarding
the duration of
therapy.
Of
course, more often than
not, one cannot base
important predictions on a single
score or attribute. The
conclusion
of therapy is more often a complex
event that has a number of
determinants.
Consequently,
the clinician might want to
obtain scores on several
other variables, such as
ego
strength,
the experience of the therapist,
marital satisfaction, and interpersonal trust. A
multivariate
prediction
model could then be
constructed and tested. A particular
caution to bear in
mind,
however,
is that even though a mu
ltiple correlation from such
an analy sis may turn
out to be
quite
high, it may well be much
lower when applied to a new sample.
This is especially true if
the
original
sample is small and the
number of predictors is large.
Further, the sample on which
the
initial
study is carried out may not
be representative of therapy patients in general.
What is true in
Kansas
may not be true in
California; what is true for
psychoanalytic therapy may not be true
for
behavior
therapy. Therefore, clinicians have to be
sure that they have
correctly weighted various
predictor
scores
before they can generalize
very far. They must
cross-validate their prediction
models using other
samples.
These
statistical techniques permit a
mechanical application that does
not involve clinical
decision
making
at all once the formulas
have been established. The
feature that distinguishes these
statistical
approaches
from clinical approaches is
that the former (no matter
what their complex
mathematical
development),
once established, can be
routinely applied by a clerk or a
computer.
The
quantitative, statistical approach, then,
requires that the clinician keep careful
records of the data,
observation
and related material so that clinical interpretations
and judgments can be quantified.
Such
careful
record keeping will permit
the clinician to go beyond informal
impressions based on
previous
experience.
With adequate records on
large enough samples, the relations
among a host of variables
can
be
assessed. Whether clinicians are
evaluating their own
performance or the performance of an
entire
clinic,
or are relating certain patient
characteristics to various diagnostic or therapeutic
outcomes,
quantified
data can play a facilitating
role. Such data enable
clinicians to evaluate their
judgment,
interpretations,
and performance.
THE
SUBJECTIVE CLINICAL APPROACH
The
clinical approach is much
more subjective, experiential, and
intuitive. Here, subjective weights
based
on
experience suffice. The emphasis is on
the application of judgment to the
individual case. The
classical
notation
is that "clinical intuition" is
not readily amenable to
analysis and quantification It is a
private
process
in which clinicians themselves
are sometimes unable to
identify the cues in a
patient's test
responses
or
verbalizations that led them to a given
conclusion or judgment.
Once,
for example, in the course of a Rorschach
administration, a patient said,
"This looks like a
Christmas
tree." What did this
mean? Perhaps nothing. Or
perhaps it indicated a career in
forestry. Or
perhaps
it suggested an underlying sadness or
depression in a person with few friends or
family with
whom
to enjoy the approaching holiday season.
In this case, the last
interpretation was later supported
by
the
patient during a discussion of his
family background. The
clinical student who had made
the
correct
interpretation in a training exercise
explained her reasoning as
follows: "It was near the
Christmas
season;
there were several references in the TAT to remote
family figures; I remembered
how I always
seem
to become a little sad
during Christmas; it suddenly popped
into my head, and I just
knew with
complete
certainty that it was
true-it simply felt
right!"
181
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
This
example illustrates several things
about clinical interpretation.
First, such interpretation
involves a
sensitive
capacity to integrate material.
The astute clinical
psychologist pays attention to
the wide range
of
events that characterize the
patient's behavior, history,
other test responses, and so on. A
clinician
must
function a bit like the
detective who takes in
everything at the scene of
the crime and
then
makes
a series of inductive or deductive
generalizations that link
these observations together.
In
addition,
there is often a willingness in
the clinician to see a bit
of him or herself in the
patient-----a
kind
of assumed similarity that enables
the clinician to utilize his
or her own experience
in
interpreting
the behavior and feelings of
another.
Unfortunately,
the presentation of this
example has been one-sided.
Little has been made of
the
clinical
student who believed that
the Christmas tree suggested
an interest in forestry .
Therefore,
we may make two additional
observations. First, there are
individual differences in
clinical
sensitivity.
Second, for every instance
of brilliant and sensitive
clinical inference, there
probably lurks
in
the unrecalled recesses of memory an
equally impressive
misinterpretation.
Clinical
interpretation, then, involves
the sensitive integration of many
sources of data into a
coherent
picture
of the patient. It also fulfills a
hypothesis-generating function that is
best of personality. But
it
behooves
responsible clinicians to make every
effort to articulate the
cues involved in their
judgments
and
to explicate the manner in
which they make the leap
from cues to conclusions. It is
not enough to
be
good clinicians. There is
also a responsibility to pass on
these skills to
others.
COMPARING
CLINICAL AND ACTUARIAL APPROACHES
Over
the years, many studies
have compared the relative
accuracy of clinical and
actuarial methods.
Let
us
now examine some of that
work.
Comparison
Studies
Sarbin
(1943) contrasted the prediction of
academic success of college freshmen
made by a clerk
employing
a regression equation with the
predictions made by several counselors.
The regression
equation
predictors
were aptitude test scores
and high school rank.
The counselors had available
to them
the
two preceding sources of data
(but without their
mathematical weighting), vocational
interest
scores,
interview data, and biographical data. Sarbin (1943)
found that the counselors
were no better than
the
regression equation in their predictions
even though they had
the benefit of much
mere
information.
Meehl
(1954) surveyed a number of the
studies available on clinical
versus statistical, prediction
and
concluded
that in "all but one
... the predictions made
actuarially [statistically] were
either
approximately
equal or superior to those
made by a clinician" .In a later
survey of additional
research,
Meehl
(1965) reaffirmed his
earlier conclusions. However,
Meehl (1954) also observed
that, in several
studies,
statistical predictions were made on
the same data from
which the regression
equations
were
developed. In short, the formulas
were not cross-validated. Such
formulas frequently show
a
marked
reduction in efficiency when they are
applied to samples different
from those used in
their
derivation.
Sawyer
(1966) regarded data
collected by interview or observation as
clinical data. He
viewed
inventory,
biographical, or clerically obtained data
as statistical or mechanical. Having
considered the
methodological
problems and the equivocal
results of the studies he examined,
Sawyer concluded that
in
combining data the mechanical
mode is superior to the
clinical mode. However, he
also
concluded
that the clinical method is
useful in the data collection
process. The clinical method
can
provide
an assessment of characteristics that
would not normally be
assessed by more
mechanical
techniques
of data collection. But once
the data (from whatever
source) are collected, they
can best be
combined
by statistical approaches.
182
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
An
example of an individual study comparing
clinical and statistical
prediction may help
further
illustrate
the nature of this controversy.
One
of the most frequently cited
studies of clinical versus statistical
prediction was reported by
Goldberg(1965)
.in this study, 13 PhD
level staff-members
and 16 predoctoral trainees were
asked to
make
judgments regarding the diagnostic status
of more than 800 patients,
based on these patients'
MMPI
scores. These judgments were
made without any contact
with the patient or any
additional
information
on the patient. Each judge
simply examined the MMPI profile (scores)
for each patient
and
then predicted whether the
patient was "psychotic" or
"neurotic." These judgments
constituted
clinical
predictions because it was left up to
each judge as to how she or
he used the MMPI
information
to
formulate a diagnosis.
In
contrast, statistical predictions
involved the application of a
variety of algorithms, in which
MMPI
scale
scores were combined (added
or subtracted), in some manner and
previously established
cutoff
scores
for psychosis versus
neurosis were used. In addition,
some statistical predictions
involved the
application
of specified decision rules based on
MMMPI high point codes or
other psychometric
signs.
A
total of 65 different quantitatively
based rules were considered.
What
were these clinical and
statistical predictions compared to in
order to assess their
accuracy? In
this
study, the criterion
diagnosis was the psychotic
versus neurotic diagnosis
provided by each
patient's
hospital or clinic. Thus,
the accuracy of each
clinician's and each
statistical algorithm's
prediction
was determined by assessing
the agreement between
predictions and the actual
criterion
diagnoses
across all cases.
A
variety of additional, updated reviews of
the studies pitting clinical
versus statistical prediction have
uniformly-demonstrated
the superiority of s t a t i s t i c a l
procedures (for example,
Dawes, 1979, 1994;
Dawes,
Faust, & Meehl. 1989;
Garb, 1998; Goldberg, 1991;
Kleinmuntz, 1990; Meehl, 1986;
Wiggins,
1973).
As stated by Meehl
(1986):
Objections
to These Findings
Dawes
(1994) has outlined several
of the major objections to large
body- of
evidence supporting the
superiority
of statistical prediction, along
with response, such
objection.
First,
critics argue that several of the
individual studies reviewed contained
research design flaws that
may
have
affected the findings. Dawes
(1994) refers to this an "argument
from a vacuum"-a possibility
is
raised,
but there is no empirical demonstration
supporting the possibility. Although
every study has
its
limitations,
it is difficult to imagine that
the opposite conclusion
(clinical prediction is superior)
is
warranted
when practically all of the
studies support statistical
prediction.
The
second objection concerns the expertise
of the judges/clinicians in these studies.
Perhaps they were
not
"true" experts, and a study
employing expert clinicians
would demonstrate the superiority
of
clinical
judgment. Although a wide
variety of judges/clinicians were used in
these studies, a number
of
studies employed recognized
"experts"-clinicians with many years of
experience performing the
predictive
task in question. There were a few
instances in which an individual
clinician performed as
well
as the statistical formula, but this
was more the exception
than the rule. Thus,
there is no
compelling
empirical evidence that
"expert" clinicians are
superior.
A
third objection is that the predictive
tasks were not representative of
prediction situations facing
clinicians
(that is, not ecologically
valid). A clinician's diagnosis may
not be based only on the
MMPI-2,
for
example, but also on an
interview with the patient.
Dawes (1994) argues, however, that
the
predictive
tasks are components of what
may go on in clinical practice
clinicians purportedly
use
the
MMPI-2 information to make predictions.
Further, several of the studies
demonstrate that additional
information
(such as interview material)
obtained and used in the
judge's clinical prediction may
actually
183
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
result
in less accurate predictions
than would be the case if
the clinician had simply
"stuck with
the
statistical formula that was
available.
Dawes
(1994) goes on to suggest that
much of the negative reaction to the
findings is a function of
our
human
need to believe in a high
degree of predictability in the world.
This appears to be both
a
cognitive
and an emotional need. People have a
built-in tendency to both seek
and see order in the
world,
and a lack of predictability in the
world is likely to result in some
degree of discomfort or
emotional
distress. However, the need
for predictability does not
prove its existence.
BIAS
IN CLINICAL JUDGMENT
Clinical
judgment suffers when bias of
any kind intrudes into
the decision-making process. Bias
exists
when
accuracy of clinical judgment or
prediction varies as a function of
some client or
patient
characteristic,
not simply when judgments
differ according to client
characteristics (Garb, 1997,
1998).
For
example, finding that a
higher percentage of women than
men are judged to suffer
from major
depression
would not indicate a bias
against women. However, finding
that a higher percentage of
women
than
men are given this
diagnosis when the same
symptoms are presented would
indicate bias.
Garb
(1997) recently reviewed the empirical
evidence for race bias,
social class bias, and
gender bias in
clinical
judgment. Interestingly, he found
that many conventionally
held beliefs about these
types of
bias
were not supported. For
example, there was little
support for the beliefs t h
at
(
1 ) l o w e r - so c i o e c o n o m i c - c l a s s p a t i e n t s a r e
judged to be more seriously disturbed
than those from
higher
socioeconomic classes or
(2)
Women patients are judged to be
more disturbed or dysfunctional than
men patients. However,
there
was
strong evidence to support the existence of several
other types of biases:
1.
Black and Hispanic patients
who have psychotic mood disorders are
more likely to be
misdiagnosed
with schizophrenia than are
similar White
patients.
2.
Even when presenting the same
constellation of symptoms, men are
more likely to be
diagnosed
as
antisocial and women are
more likely to be diagnosed as
histrionic.
3.
Middle-class patients are
more likely to be referred for
psychotherapy than lower-class patients.
4.
Black patients are more likely to be
prescribed antipsychotic medications than
members of other
racial
groups, even when the Black patients are
not more psychotic.
Garb
(1997) made the following
recommendations to help clinicians
overcome these and other
bias
(1)
Be aware of and sensitive to
the biases that have
been documented in the
literature.
(2)
Attend to the diagnostic criteria in
diagnostic manuals.
(3)
Whenever possible, use
statistical prediction rules instead of
clinical judgment or prediction
EXPERIENCE
AND TRAINING
To
date, empirical evidence
does not support the
position that increased
clinical experience results
in
increased
accuracy in prediction (Dawes,
1994; Garb, 1989, 1998).
This seems to fly in the
face of
conventional
wisdom. Why is it that we do
not see evidence for
the effect of clinical
experience in
clinical
psychology and other mental health fields?
There are several
possibilities (Dawes,
1994).
First,
the accuracy of predictions is limited by
the available measures and
methods that are used as
aids
184
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
in
the prediction process. If scores
from psychological tests,
for example, are not
strongly correlated
with
the
criterion of interest (that is,
highly valid), then it is unlikely
one could ever observe an
effect for
clinical
experience. The accuracy of predictions
will remain modest at best
and will not depend on
how
"clinically
experienced" the clinician is.
Second,
we often cannot define precisely
what we are trying to
predict (for example,
"abusive
personality"),
and no gold standards for
our criteria exist to enable us to
assess objectively the
accuracy of
our
predictions. As a result, true feedback
is impossible, and diagnosticians are
not able to profit
from
experience.
Third,
we tend to remember our accurate
predictions and to forget our
inaccurate ones.
Therefore,
more
experience in the prediction
process does not necessarily
lead to increased accuracy because
the
feedback
that is incorporated is
incomplete.
As
for the virtue of receiving
specific types of professional
training, there is not much
evidence to
suggest
that one profession is
superior to another in making accurate
diagnostic judgments. For
example,
even in differentiating psychological
symptoms that are masking
medical disorders from
those
without
underlying medical disorders,
medical and non medical
practitioners did not differ
in their
accuracy
(Sanchez & Kahn,
1991).
All
of this research is somewhat sobering
for the field of clinical
psychology. However, it is
our
professional
responsibility to be aware of the
limits of our predictive
ability and not to promote
the "myth
of
experience." One thing is sure.
Clinicians will continue to make
decisions-they have no
choice.
The
important thing is to ensure
that clinical psychologists are as
well prepared as' they
can be, as well
as
to train clinical psychologists to use
the-best available measures
and techniques for a given
prediction
situation.
CONCLUSION
Given
the current state of
affairs, the following conclusions
regarding the relative strengths of
clinical and
actuarial
methods seem warranted.
The
clinical
approach is especially
valuable when:
1.
Information
is needed
about
areas or events for which no adequate
tests are available. In
this case,
the
research fails to offer any
evidence that the data-gathering
function of the clinician
can be
replaced
by a machine.
2.
Rare,
unusual events of a highly
individualized nature are to be
predicted' or judged.
Regression
equations
or other formulas cannot be developed to
handle such events, and
clinical judgment is the
only
recourse.
3.
The clinical
judgments involve
instances for which no
statistical equations have
been developed.
The
vast majority of instances, in effect,
fall into this category.
The day-to-day decisions of
the
clinician
are such that the
availability of a useful equation
would itself be a one and unusual
event.
4
.
T h e role of
unforeseen circumstances
could negate the efficiency of a
formula.
For
example, a
formula
might very easily outstrip
the performance of a clinician in
predicting suitability for
hospital
discharge.
In the role of data gatherer,
however, the clinician might
unearth important data from
a
patient
that would negate an otherwise
perfectly logical statistical
prediction.
The
statistical
approach is especially
valuable when:
185
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
1.
The
outcome to be predicted is objective
and specific. For example,
the statistical
approach
w
i l l b e e sp e c i a l l y e f f ec t i v e i n p r ed i c t i n g
grades, successful discharge,
vocational success,
and
similar objective
outcomes.
2.
The
outcomes for large, heterogeneous
samples are involved, and
interest in the individual
case is
minimal. Having
a statistical formul a to predict
how many of 50,000 men will
receive
dishonorable
discharges from the Army will be
highly useful to the Army,
though less so
for
the clinician who is dealing
with Private Smith.
3.
There is
reason to be particularly concerned
about human judgmental error
or bias.
Fatigue,
boredom,
bias, and a h o st o f o t her h u
man failings can be responsible
for clinical error.
Oft
en , su ch effect s are ran do m 'a n d u
np redictable. Formulas, equations,
and computers
never
become tired, bored, or
biased.
186
Table of Contents:
|
|||||