|
|||||
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
LESSON
17
THE
ASSESSMENT OF INTELLIGENCE
The
history of clinical psychology is
inextricably tied to the assessment of
intelligence. Without the
success
in this and related
assessment enterprises, there might
not have been a field of
clinical
psychology.
As the years passed, however;
clinicians became increasingly interested
in the more
"glamorous"
aspects of the profession, such as
therapy. Assessment began to
take a back seat, and
technicians
started to become the assessors, as
they had been prior to
World War II. However; all
this is
beginning
to change. Not only is the
value of assessment being rediscovered,
but intelligence tests
in
particular
remain prominent in the clinician's
arsenal of assessment devices.
THE
CONCEPT OF INTELLIGENCE
The
concept of intelligence has
presented clinical psychology
with one of its greatest
dilemmas. On the
one
hand, psychologists have been pressured
for almost 100 years to
quantify individual differences
in
intellectual
functioning. On the other hand,
intelligence has remained one of the most
difficult and
controversial
psychological construct to define and
measure. The impetus to define and
quantify
intelligence
comes from both practical
and scientific forces. The
practical significance lies in
the
potential
use of measures of intelligence in
predicting academic and work
potential and achievement.
From
a scientific perspective, precise
understanding of how to measure
intelligence can contribute
to
our
comprehension of an important aspect of human
behavior and functioning.
Two
issues that have plagued psychologists
from the beginning are still
not resolved. First, exactly
what
is
meant by the term intelligence?
Second,
how do we develop valid instruments
for measuring it? We
will
address both questions. But
first we need to review the psychometric
concepts of reliability and
validity.
RELIABILTY
AND VALIDITY
RELIABILITY
With
regard to psychological tests,
reliability refers to the consistency with
which individuals respond
to
test
stimuli. There are several ways of
evaluating reliability.
First,
there is test-retest
reliability-the
extent
to which an individual makes
similar responses to the
same
test
stimuli on repeated occasions. If
each time we test a person
we get different responses, the
test data
may
not be very useful. In some
instances, clients may
remember on the second occasion
their responses
from
the first time. Or they may
develop a kind of "test-wiseness"
from
the first test that
influences
their
scores the second time
around. In still other
cases, clients may rehearse
between testing occasions
or
show practice effects. For all these
reasons, another gauge of reliability is
sometimes used-
equivalent-forms
reliability. Here,
equivalent or parallel forms of a test
are developed to avoid
the
preceding
problems.
Sometimes
it is too expensive (in time
or money) to develop an equivalent
form or it is difficult or
impossible
to be sure the forms are
really equivalent. Under
such circumstances, or when retesting is
not
practical,
assessing split-half
reliability is
a possibility. This means
that a test is divided into
halves
(usually
odd-numbered items versus even-numbered items), and
participants' scores on the two
halves
are
compared. Split-half reliability also
serves as one possible index of a
test's internal
consistency: Do
the
items on the test appear to be measuring
the same thing? That is, are
the items highly correlated with
each
other? The preferred method of assessing
internal consistency reliability
involves computing the
aver-age
of all possible split-half correlations
for a given test.
134
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Another
aspect of reliability, inter-rater
or
inter-judge
reliability is
the index of the degree of
agreement
between
two or more raters or judgers as to the
level of a trait that is
present or the presence/absence of
a
features or diagnosis.
VALIDITY
In
general, validity refers to the extent to
which as assessment technique
measures what it is supposed
to
measure.
There are several forms of
validity.
Content
Validity indicates the
degree to which a group of
test items actually covers the
various aspects
of
the variable under
study.
Predictive
Validity is demonstrated
when test scores accurately
predict some behavior or
event in the
future.
Concurrent
Validity involves
relating today's test scores
to a concurrent criterion.
Finally,
Construct
Validity is shown when
test scores relate to other
measures or behaviors in a
logical,
theoretically
expected fashion.
DEFINITION
OF INTELLIGENCE
Intelligence
is a hypothetical
construct; that is,
intelligence is a concept that
exists only in the way
that
psychologists
and the public chose to define
it. You cannot touch intelligence
nor can you
directly
observe
it. You can only observe the
consequences of intelligence as they
are reflected in the
behavior
and
performance of individual.
There
is no universally accepted definition of
intelligence. However, over the
years, most have
fallen
into
one of three classes:
1.
Definitions
that emphasize adjustment or adaptation
to the environment-adaptability to
new
situations,
the capacity to deal with a range of
situations.
2.
Definitions
that focus on the ability to learn-on
educability in the broad sense of the
terms.
3.
Definitions
that emphasize abstract
thinking the ability to use a
wide range of symbols
and
concepts,
the ability to use both
verbal and numerical symbols.
To
illustrate a little of the long-standing
diversity of definitions, consider the
following examples:
Intelligence
is the aggregate or global capacity of
the individual to act purpose-fully, to
think rationally,
and
to deal effectively with his
environment. (Wechsler, 1939)
As
a concept, intelligence refers to the whole
class of cognitive behaviors
which reflects an individual's
capacity
to solve problems with insight, to
adapt himself to new situations, to
think abstractly, and
to
profit
from his experience. (Robinson & Robinson,
1965)
Intelligence
is expressed in terms of adaptive,
goal-directed behavior the subset of
such behavior that
is
labeled
"intelligent" seems to be determined in
large part by cultural or
societal norms. (Sternberg
&
Salter,
1982)
Intelligence
is a very general mental
capability that, among other
things, involves the ability to
reason,
plan,
solve problems, and think
has very few traits in
common-they resemble the prototype
along
135
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
different
dimensions. Thus, there is no such
thing as chariness - resemblance is an
external fact and
not
an
internal essence. There can be no
process-based definition of intelligence,
because it is not a
unitary
quality.
It is a resemblance between two
individuals, one real and the
other prototypical. (Spearman)
THEORIES
OF INTELLIGENCE
There
have been many theoretical
approaches to the understanding of
intelligence. These
include
psychometric
theories, developmental theories, neuropsychological
theories, and information-processing
theories.
We present only a brief
overview of several leading theories
here.
FACTOR
ANALYTICAL APPROACHES
Spearman
(1927), the father of factor analysis,
posited the existence of a g
factor
(general intelligence)
and
S
factors
(specific intelligence). The
elements that tests have in common
are represented by g,
whereas
the elements unique to a given
test are S
factors.
Basically, however, Spearman's
message,
buttressed
by factor analytic evidence, was
that intelligence is a broad,
generalized entity.
A
number of individuals took issue
with Spearman's contentions,
including E. L. Thorndike and L.
L.
Thurstone.
For example, Thurstone
(1938) presented evidence (based on a
factor analysis of 57 separate
tests
that had been administered to
240 participants) for a
series of "group" factors rather than
the
almighty
g
factor.
Ultimately, Thurstone described
seven group factors which he
labeled number, word
fluency,
verbal meaning, perceptual speed,
space, reasoning, and memory.
CATTELL'S
THEORY
The
work of R. B. Cattell (1987)
emphasizes the centrality of
g.
At
the same time, Cattell has
offered a
tentative
list of 17 primary ability
concepts. He has described two
important second-order factors
that
seem
to represent a partitioning of Spearman's
& into two components:
fluid
ability (the
person's geneti-
cally
based intellectual capacity)
and crystallized
ability (the
capacities, tapped by the usual
standardized
intelligence test that can
be attributed to culture-based learning).
Essentially, Cattell's
approach
might be described as a hierarchical
model of intelligence.
GUILFORD'S
THEORY: The
views of Guilford (1967) were
quite different from those
of Cattell,
Spearman,
Thurstone, and most other
psychometricians. Guilford proposed a Structure
of the Intellect (S
01)
model and
then used a variety of
statistical and factor
analytic techniques to test it.
Whereas other
psychometric
approaches generally attempted to infer a
model, Guilford used the
model as a guide in
generating
data.
Guilford
reasoned that the components of
intelligence could be organized
into three dimensions:
operations,
contents, and products. The operations
are cognition, memory,
divergent production
(constructing
logical alternatives), Convergent
production (constructing logic-tight
arguments), and
evaluation.
The content dimension involves the
areas of information in which the
operations are
performed:
figural, symbolic, semantic, and
behavioral. Finally, when a
particular mental operation
is
applied
to a specific type of content, there are
six possible products: units, classes,
systems, relations,
transformations,
and implications. If we contemplate all possible
combinations, we arrive at
120
separate
intellectual abilities. Perhaps
the most widely held
reservation about Guilford's approach is
that
it
is taxonomy or classification rather than
a theory.
RECENT
DEVELOPMENTS:Traditionally,
intelligence tests have been
constructed to assess
what
we
know or can do. Recent
approaches, however, have begun to take
on a highly cognitive or
in-
formation-processing
look. For example, some
researchers try to describe a
person's moment-by-
moment
attempts to solve a problem-from the
moment a stimulus registers to the
person's verbal or
motor
response.. This is a more dynamic
view of intelligence than the
older theories of mental
components.
136
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Some
of these researchers have focused on
speed of information processing
and others on strategies
of
processing.
A number of levels of processing have
been studied, including
speed of processing, speed
in
making
choices in response to stimuli, and
speed with which individuals
can extract various aspects
of
language
from their long-term memory.
But many problems and questions remain
(Gardner, 1983). Is
there
a central processing mechanism
for information? How do the
processing elements change as
the
person
develops? Are there general problem-solving
skills or merely skills
specific to certain
ability
areas?
Perhaps time will
tell.
Gardner
(1983)
has described a theory of
multiple
intelligences. Human
intellectual competence
involves
a set of problem-solving skills
that enable the person to resolve
problems or difficulties.
Sometimes
this results in the potential
for acquiring new
information. Gardner suggests
that there is a
family
of six intelligences: linguistic,
musical, logical-mathematical, spatial,
bodily-kinesthetic, and
personal.
For example, the personal refers both to
access to one's own feeling
life and to the ability to
notice
and make distinctions among other
individuals. A major criticism of
Gardner's theory is that
some
of his proposed "intelligences"
may
be better conceptualized as "talents"
than
as forms of
intelligence.
Nevertheless, Gardner's views have attracted a great
deal of attention from
psychologists
and
educators alike.
To
cite another example of a theory of
multiple forms of intelligence,
Sternberg
(1985,
1991) has
proposed
a triarchic
theory of intelligence. He
maintains that people
function on the basis of three
aspects
of intelligence: componential,
experiential, and contextual.
This
approach deemphasizes speed and
accuracy of performance. Instead, the
emphasis is on planning
responses
and monitoring them. The componential
aspect refers to analytical thinking;
high scores
would
characterize the person who is a
good test-taker. The
experiential aspect relates to
creative
thinking
and characterizes the person who
can take separate elements
of experience and combine them
insightfully.
Finally, the contextual aspect is
seen in the person who is "street
smart"-one who knows
how
to play the game and can successfully
manipulate the environment.
According
to Sternberg, a person's performance is governed by
these three aspects of intelligence.
Other
investigators
are particularly interested in social
competence as an aspect of intelligence
(Sternberg &
Wagner,
1986). However, whether all
the foregoing can account
for individual differences or is
just a
theory
of cognition is debatable.
Although
Spearman, Thurstone, and others
may seem to have given way
to Cattell, Guilford,
Gardner,
or
Sternberg, clinicians' day-to-day use of
tests suggests that they
have not really outgrown the
g factor
of
Spearman or the group factors of
Thurstone. The whole notion
of a single IQ score that
can represent
the
individual's intelligence strongly
implies that we are trying
to discover how much
g the
person has.
At
the same time, however, most
current intelligence tests
are composed of subtests, so
that the total IQ
represents
some average of subtest
scores.
This
implies that, to some extent
at least, we have also accepted
Turnstone's group factors. We seem
to
want
to identify and quantify how
much intelligence the person
has, yet we cannot escape the
belief that
intelligence
is somehow patterned-that two
people may have the same
overall IQ score and still
differ in
specific
abilities. Thus, it would appear
that practicing clinicians
think more in line with
Spearman or
Thurstone
and are as yet little
affected by the recent information
processing developments.
HISTORY
OF INTELLIGENCE TESTING
Two
important historical developments in the
latter half of the nineteenth
century greatly influenced
the
ultimate
introduction of measures of intelligence
(Thorndike, 1997). First,
compulsory education in the
United
States and other countries resulted in a
very diverse student body.
Many students came
from
"uneducated"
families or families that
did not speak the native
tongue. As a result, the failure rate
in
137
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
schools
shot up dramatically. In order to
preserve resources, there was
pressure to identify those
most
likely
to succeed in school. Second,
psychological scientists believed, and
ultimately demonstrated, that
mental
abilities could be measured.
Although early attempts
focused primarily on measures of
sensory
acuity
and reaction time (for
example, Francis Galton, James
McKeen Cattell), the
groundwork was laid
in
place.
Alfred
Binet and his collaborator,
Theodore Simon, became
leaders in the intelligence testing
movement
when
they devised the Binet-Simon test to
identify individual differences in
mental functioning.
Binet's
original
purpose was to develop an
objective method of identifying those
truly lacking in
academic
ability
(as opposed to those with
behavior problems). Like others of the
day, Binet and Simon
regarded
intelligence
as a "faculty" that was
inherited, although they
also spoke of it as affected by
training and
opportunity.
With the interest in quantifying
intellectual performance and with the
continuing growth of
compulsory
education in Europe and North
America, intelligence testing
became firmly entrenched
(Thorndike,
1997).
Institutions
such as schools, industries,
military forces, and governments were, by
their nature,
interested
in individual differences (such as
levels of intelligence) that
might affect performance in
those
settings;
therefore, intelligence testing
prospered (Herrnstein & Murray,
1994).For many years,
the
critical
importance and widespread use of
intelligence tests went
largely unchallenged. However, by
the
end
of the 1960s, everyone seemed to be
attacking the validity of these
tests. Basically, the argument
was
that such tests discriminate
through the inclusion of unfair
items.
As
a result of a lengthy civil
rights suit (Larvy
P
v. Wilson
Riles) begun in 1971,
the California State
Board
of Education in 1975 imposed a moratorium
on the use of intelligence tests to
assess disabilities
in
African Americans. The court
held that IQ testing is
prejudicial to African American
children and
tends
to place them, without real
justification, in allegedly stigmatizing
programs for
cognitively
impaired
individuals.
Others
(for example, N. Lambert,
1981) have disputed the court's
judgment, however. Some
African
Americans
contemplated a court challenge of the
ruling, claiming it assumed
that African
Americans
would
do poorly on the tests. Still
others argued that IQ testing is
not a social evil but the
principal
means
by which we can right the
wrongs imposed upon minorities by a
devastating environment.
The
most widely
used intelligence tests in
the United States are those
originally developed by
psychologist
David Wechsler during the 1940's and
1950's.building on existing tests of the
day--
including
the Standford-Binet the Army Alpha and
Beta tests, and the Bellevue
intelligence scale--
Wechsler
first developed an individual
test in intelligence for adults.
Followed by a similarly structured
test
for school-age children to age
16, and finally, a test for
pre-school-age children.
These
tests were influenced by Wechsler's
belief that there is a total or
global level of
intellectual
capacity
that can be measured-thus;
these tests yield a score
that represents the person's
overall
intelligence.
the tests developed by Wechsler
also reflect the geographic metaphor of
intelligence
described
by Sternberg(1990).Wechsler's tests more than
any others have shaped
psychologist's
perception
of intellectual functioning as comprised of
separate but related verbal
and performance(non-
verbal
abilities).
Wechsler
(1939) emphasized that an IQ
test measures functional
intelligence,
not intelligence it
self.
Functional
intelligence is influenced by
nonintellectual factors including
motivation, configuration of
specific
abilities, and emotional adjustment.
According to Wechsler score on an IQ
test is a reflection of
what
one has learned, and which is a
function of the opportunities to which
one has been exposed
and
one's
ability to take advantage of those
opportunities. The subsets on Wechsler's
test represent
samples
of
behavior but they are
not exhaustive.
138
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
The
need for continued
adaptation of intelligence testing is
represented in the history of the
Wechsler
scales,
because all three versions have undergone substantial
revisions since their
inception and even
after
Wechsler's death. These changes have
taken two forms: changes in
the items of the tests to make
them
more current and appropriate for
new generations, and the testing of new
normative samples to
provide
up-to-date sources for
normative comparison in generating
scores.
CONCLUSION
There
is little question that
intelligence tests have been
misused at times in ways that have
penalized
minorities.
There is also little doubt
that some tests have
contained certain items that have
adversely
affected
the performance of some minorities. We
should, therefore, do everything we
can to develop
better
tests and to administer and interpret
them in a sensitive fashion. However,
banning tests seems
an
inappropriate
cure that may ultimately
harm the very people who
need help.
139
Table of Contents:
|
|||||