|
|||||
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
LESSON
13
CAUSES OF
MENTAL ILLNESOVERVIEW OF
ETIOLOGY
The
precise causes (etiology) of
most mental disorders are
not known. But the key
word in this
statement
is precise. The precise
causes of most mental
disorders--or, indeed, of mental
health--may
not
be known, but the broad
forces that shape them are
known: these are biological,
psychological, and
social/cultural
factors. What is most important to go
over is that the causes of
health and disease
are
generally
viewed as a product of the interplay or
interaction between biological,
psychological, and
sociocultural
factors. This is true for
all health and illness,
including mental health and
mental illness.
For
instance, diabetes and schizophrenia alike
are viewed as the result of
interactions between
biological,
psychological, and sociocultural
influences. With these disorders, a
biological predisposition
is
necessary but not sufficient
to explain their occurrence (Barondes,
1993). For other disorders,
a
psychological
or sociocultural cause may be
necessary, but again not
sufficient.
The
brain and behavior are
inextricably linked by the plasticity of
the nervous system. The brain is
the
organ
of mental function; psychological
phenomena have their origin in
that complex organ.
Psychological
and sociocultural phenomena are
represented in the brain through
memories and learning,
which
involve structural changes in the
neurons and neuronal circuits.
Yet neuroscience does not
intend
to
reduce all phenomena to neurotransmission
or to reinterpret them in a new language of
synapses,
receptors,
and circuits. Psychological and
sociocultural events and phenomena
continue to have
meaning
for mental health and mental
illness. It is still meaningful to
speak of the interaction of
biological
and psychological and sociocultural factors in
health and illness.
BIOPSYCHOSOCIAL
MODEL OF DISEASE
The
modern view that many factors
interact to produce disease may be
attributed to the seminal work of
George
L. Engel, who in 1977 put
forward the Biopsychosocial Model of
Disease (Engel,
1977).
Engel's
model is a framework, rather than a
set of detailed hypotheses,
for understanding health
and
disease.
To many scientists, the model lacks
sufficient specificity to make
predictions about the
given
cause
or causes of any one disorder. Scientists
want to find out what is the
specifically
contribution
of
different
factors (e.g., genes, parenting, culture,
stressful events) and how they operate.
But the purpose
of
the biopsychosocial model is to take a
broad view, to assert that
simply looking at biological
factors
alone--which
had been the prevailing view of
disease at the time Engel
was writing--is not
sufficient to
explain
health and illness.
According
to Engel's model, biopsychosocial factors
are involved in the causes,
manifestation, course,
and
outcome of health and disease,
including mental disorders. The
model certainly fits with
common
experience.
Few people with a condition
such as heart disease or diabetes,
for instance, would
dispute
the
role of stress in aggravating
their condition. Research
bears this out and reveals
many other
relationships
between stress and disease (Cohen &
Herbert, 1996; Baum & Posluszny,
1999).
One
single factor in isolation--biological,
psychological, or social--may weigh
heavily or hardly at
all,
depending
on the behavioral trait or mental
disorder. That is, the relative
importance or role of any
one
factor
in causation often varies. For example, a
personality trait like
extroversion is linked strongly
to
genetic
factors, according to identical twin
studies (Plomin et al.,
1994). Similarly, schizophrenia is
linked
strongly to genetic factors, also
according to twin
studies.
But
this does not mean
that genetic factors completely
preordain or fix the nature of the disorder
and
that
psychological and social factors are
unimportant. These social factors modify
expression and
outcome
of disorders. Likewise, some mental
disorders, such as post-traumatic stress
disorder (PTSD),
99
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
are
clearly caused by exposure to an
extremely stressful event,
such as rape, combat, natural
disaster etc.
Yet
not everyone develops PTSD
after such exposure. On average, about 9
percent do (Breslau et al.,
1998),
but estimates are higher
for particular types of trauma. For
women who are victims of
crime, one
study
found the prevalence of PTSD in a representative
sample of women to be 26 percent (Resnick
et
al.,
1993).
The
likelihood of developing PTSD is
related to pre-trauma vulnerability (in
the form of genetic,
biological,
and personality factors), magnitude of the
stressful event, preparedness
for the event, and the
quality
of care after the event
(Shalev, 1996). The relative
roles of biological, psychological, or
social
factors
also may vary across
individuals and across
stages of the life span. In
some people, for
example,
depression
arises primarily as a result of exposure
to stressful life events,
whereas in others the foremost
cause
of depression is genetic
predisposition.
UNDERSTANDING
CORRELATION, CAUSATION, AND
CONSEQUENCES
Any
discussion of the etiology of mental
health and mental illness
needs to distinguish three key
terms:
correlation,
causation, and consequences. These
terms are often confused. All
too frequently a
biological
change in the brain (a lesion) is
purported to be the "cause" of a mental
disorder, based on
finding
an association between the lesion and a
mental disorder. The fact is
that any simple
association--or
correlation--cannot and does not, by
itself, mean causation. The
lesion could be a
correlate,
a cause of, or an effect of the
mental disorder.
When
researchers begin to tease apart
etiology, they usually start
by noticing correlations. A
correlation
is
an association or linkage of two (or
more) events. A correlation simply
means that the events
are
linked
in some way. Finding a
correlation between stressful life
events and depression would
prompt
more
research on causation. Does stress
cause depression? Does
depression cause stress? Or
are they
both
caused by an unidentified factor?
These would be the questions guiding
research. But, with
co-
relational
research, several steps are
needed before causation can be
established.
If
a co-relational study shows
that a stressful event is
associated with an increased
probability for
depression
and that the stress usually
precedes depression's onset, then
stress is called a "risk
factor"
for
depression. Risk factors are
biological, psychological, or
sociocultural variables that
increase the
probability
for developing a disorder
and antedate its onset.
For each mental disorder,
there are likely to
be
multiple risk factors, which
are woven together in a
complex chain of causation. Some
risk factors
may
carry more weight than others, and the
interaction of risk factors may be
additive or synergistic.
Establishing
causation of mental health and mental
illness is extremely difficult. Studies
in the form of
randomized,
controlled experiments provide the
strongest evidence of causation. The
problem is that
experimental
research in humans may be
logistically, ethically, or financially
impossible. Co-relational
research
in humans has thus provided
much of what is known about
the etiology of mental disorders.
Yet
co-relational research is not as strong
as experimental research in permitting
inferences about
causality.
The establishment of a cause and
effect relationship requires multiple
studies and requires
judgment
about the weight of all the evidence.
Multiple co-relational studies
can be used to support
causality,
when, for example,
evaluating the effectiveness of clinical
treatments.
But,
when studying etiology,
co-relational studies are, if possible,
best combined with evidence
of
biological
plausibility. This means
that co-relational findings
should fit with biological,
chemical, and
physical
findings about mechanisms of
action relating to cause and
effect. Biological plausibility
is
often
established in animal models of disease.
That is why researchers seek
animal models in which to
study
causation. In mental health research,
there are some animal
models--such as for anxiety
and
hyperactivity--but
a major problem is the difficulty of
finding animal models that
simulate what is
often
uniquely human functioning. The
search for animal models,
however, is very
important.
100
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Consequences
are defined as the later
outcomes of a disorder. For
example, the most
serious
consequence
of depression in older people is
increased mortality from
either suicide or medical
illness.
The
basis for this relationship
is not fully known.
Putting
this all together, the
biopsychosocial model holds
that biological, psychological, or social
factors
may
be causes, correlates, and/or
consequences in relation to mental
health and mental illness.
A
stressful
life event, such as
receiving the news of a diagnosis of
cancer, offers a graphic
example of a
psychological
event that causes immediate
biological changes and later
has psychological,
biological,
and
social consequences.
When
a patient receives news of the
cancer diagnosis, the brain's sensory
cortex simultaneously
registers
the information (a correlate) and sets in
motion biological changes
that cause the heart to
pound
faster. The patient may
experience an almost immediate fear of death
that may later escalate
to
anxiety
or depression. This certainly
has been established for
breast cancer patients (Farragher,
1998).
Anxiety
and depression are, in this
case, consequences of the cancer
diagnosis, although the exact
mechanisms
are not understood. Being
anxious or depressed may
prompt further changes in
behavior,
such
as social withdrawal. So there may be social
consequences to the diagnosis as well.
This example
is
designed to lay out some of the
complexity of the biopsychosocial
model applied to mental
health and
mental
illness.
BIOLOGICAL
INFLUENCES ON MENTAL HEALTH AND MENTAL
ILLNESS
There
are far-reaching biological and
physical influences on mental
health and mental illness.
The major
categories
are genes, infections,
physical trauma, nutrition, hormones, and
toxins (e.g., lead). We
will
focus
on the first two categories--genes and
infections--for these are among the
most exciting and
intensive
areas of research relating to
biological influences on mental
health and mental
illness
THE
GENETICS OF BEHAVIOR AND MENTAL
ILLNESS
That
genes influence behavior,
normal and abnormal, has
long been established. Genes
influence
behavior
across the animal spectrum,
from the lowly fruit-fly all
the way to humans. Sorting
out which
genes
are involved and determining
how they influence behavior
present the greatest
challenge.
Research
suggests that many mental
disorders arise in part from
defects not in single genes,
but in
multiple
genes. However, none of the genes
has yet been pinpointed
for common mental disorders
(National
Institute of Mental Health [NIMH],
1998). The human genome contains
approximately 80,000
genes
that occupy approximately 5 percent of
the DNA sequences of the human genome.
The human
genome
project have provided an initial
rough draft version of the
entire sequence of the human
genome,
and in the ensuing years, gaps in the
sequence will be closed,
errors will be corrected, and
the
precise
boundaries of genes will be
identified.
In
parallel, clinical medicine is
studying the aggregation of human disease
in families. This
effort
includes
the study of mental illness,
most notably schizophrenia, bipolar
disorder (manic
depressive
illness),
early onset depression, autism,
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,
anorexia nervosa, panic
disorder,
and a number of other mental disorders.
From studying how these
disorders run in families,
and
from initial molecular
analyses of the genomes of these
families, we have learned that
heredity--
that
is, genes--plays a role in the transmission of
vulnerability of all the aforementioned
disorders from
generation
to generation.
But
we have also learned that the transmission of
risk is not simple. Certain
human diseases such as
Huntington's
disease and cystic fibrosis
result from the transmission of a
mutation--that is, a
deleteriously
altered gene sequence--at one
location in the human genome. In these
diseases, a single
mutation
has everything to say about
whether one will get the illness.
The transmission of a trait due to
a
101
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
single
gene in the human genome is called
Mendelian transmission, after the
Austrian monk, Gregor
Mendel,
who was the first to develop
principles of modern genetics and who
studied traits due to
single
genes.
When
a single gene determines the presence or
absence of a disease or other
trait, genes are rather
easy
to
discover on the basis of modern methods.
Indeed, for almost all
Mendelian disorders across
medicine
that
affect more than a few
people, the genes already have
been identified. In contrast to
Mendelian
disorders,
to our knowledge, all mental
illnesses and all normal
variants of behavior are
genetically
complex.
What this means is that no
single gene or even a combination of
genes dictates whether
someone
will have an illness or a particular
behavioral trait.
Rather,
mental illness appears to
result from the interaction of
multiple genes that confer
risk, and this
risk
is converted into illness by the
interaction of genes with
environmental factors. The implications
for
science
are, first, that no gene is
equivalent to fate for
mental illness. This gives
us hope that modifiable
environmental
risk factors can eventually be
identified and become targets
for prevention
efforts.
In
addition, we recognize that genes,
while significant in their
aggregate contribution to risk,
may each
contribute
only a small increment, and,
therefore, will be difficult to
discover. However, using
new
technologies
rising from the Human Genome
Project, we will know the
sequence of each human
gene
and
the common variants for each
gene throughout the human
race. With this information,
combined
with
modern technologies, we will in the
coming years identify genes
that confer risk of specific
mental
illnesses.
This
information will be of the highest
importance for several reasons.
First, genes are the
blueprints of
cells.
The products of genes, proteins,
work together in pathways or in building
cellular structures, so
that
finding variants within
genes will suggest pathways
that can be targets of
opportunity for the
development
of new therapeutic interventions.
Genes will also be important
clues to what goes wrong
in
the
brain when a disease occurs.
For example, once we know
that a certain gene is
involved in risk of a
particular
mental illness such as schizophrenia or
autism, we can ask at what
time during the
development
of the brain that particular
gene is active and in which cells and
circuits the gene is
expressed.
This will give us clues to
critical times for intervention in a
disease process and
information
about
what it is that goes
wrong.
Finally,
genes will provide tools
for those scientists who
are searching for environmental
risk factors.
Information
from genetics will tell us
at what age environmental cofactors in
risk must be active,
and
genes
will help us identify
homogeneous populations for
studies of treatment and of
prevention.
Heritability
refers to how much genetics
contributes to the variation of a disease
or trait in a population
at
a given point in time
(Plomin et al., 1997). Once
a disorder is established as running in
families, the
next
step is to determine its
heritability, then its mode
of transmission, and, lastly, its
location through
genetic
mapping.
One
powerful method for estimating
heritability is through twin
studies. Twin studies often
compare the
frequency
with which identical versus
fraternal twins display a
disorder. Since identical twins
are from
the
same fertilized egg, they
share the exact genetic inheritance.
Fraternal twins are from
separate eggs
and
thereby share only 50 percent of
their genetic inheritance. If a
disorder is heritable, identical
twins
should
have a higher rate of concordance--the expression of
the trait by both members of a
twin pair--
than
fraternal twins.
Such
studies, however, do not
furnish information about
which or how many genes
are involved. They
just
can be used to estimate
heritability. For example, the
heritability of bipolar disorder,
according to
the
most rigorous twin study, is
about 59 percent, although other
estimates vary (NIMH, 1998).
The
heritability
of schizophrenia is estimated, on the basis of twin
studies, at a somewhat higher
level
(NIMH,
1998). Even with a high
level of heritability, however, it is
essential to point out
that
102
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
environmental
factors (e.g., psychosocial environment, nutrition,
health care access) can
play a
significant
role in the severity and
course of a disorder.
Another
point is that environmental factors
may even protect against the disorder
developing in the first
place.
Even with the relatively
high heritability of schizophrenia, the
median concordance rate among
identical
twins is 46 percent (NIMH, 1998), meaning
that in over half of the
cases, the second twin
does
not
manifest schizophrenia even though he or she
has the same genes as the
affected twin. This
implies
that
environmental factors exert a significant
role in the onset of
schizophrenia.
INFECTIOUS
INFLUENCES
It
has been known since the
early part of the 20th
century that infectious
agents can penetrate into
the
brain
where they can cause mental
disorders. A highly common mental
disorder of unknown etiology
at
the
turn of the century, termed "general
paresis," turned out to be a late
manifestation of syphilis.
The
sexually transmitted infectious
agent--Treponema pallidum--first caused
symptoms in
reproductive
organs and then, sometimes
years later, migrated to the
brain where it led to
neurosyphilis.
Neurosyphilis
was manifest by neurological
deterioration (including psychosis),
paralysis, and later
death.
With the wide availability of
penicillin after World War
II, neurosyphilis was virtually
eliminated
(Barondes,
1993).
Neurosyphilis
may be thought of as a disease of the
past (at least in the
developed world), but
dementia
associated
with infection by the human
immunodeficiency virus (HIV) is
certainly not.
HIV-associated
dementia
continues to encumber HIV-infected
individuals worldwide. HIV infection
penetrates into the
brain,
producing a range of progressive cognitive and
behavioral impairments.
Early
symptoms include impaired
memory and concentration,
psychomotor slowing, and apathy.
Later
symptoms,
usually appearing years
after infection, include
global impairments marked by
mutism,
incontinence,
and paraplegia (Navia et al.,
1986).
The
prevalence of HIV-associated dementia varies, with
estimates ranging from 15 percent to
44
percent
of patients with HIV infection (Grant et
al., 1987; McArthur et al.,
1993). The high end of
this
estimate
includes patients with subtle
neuropsychological abnormalities. What is
remarkable about HIV-
associated
dementia is that it appears to be caused
not by direct infection of neurons,
but by infection of
immune
cells known as macrophages that enter the
brain from the blood.
The macrophages
indirectly
cause
dysfunction and death in nearby neurons
by releasing soluble toxins (Epstein
& Gendelman,
1993).
Besides
HIV-associated dementia and neurosyphilis,
other mental disorders are
caused by infectious
agents.
They include herpes simplex
encephalitis, measles encephalomyelitis,
rabies encephalitis,
and
chronic
meningitis(Kaplan & Sadock, 1998).
More recently, research has
uncovered an infectious
etiology
to one form of obsessive-compulsive
disorder, as explained
below.
PANDAS
In
the late 1980s, it was discovered that
some children with obsessive-compulsive
disorder (OCD)
experienced
a sudden onset of symptoms
soon after a streptococcal
pharyngitis (Garvey et
al., 1998).
The
symptoms were classic for
OCD--concerns about contamination,
spitting compulsions, and
extremely
excessive hoarding--but the abrupt
onset was unusual. Further
study of these children led
to
the
identification of a new classification of
OCD called PANDAS. This
acronym stands for pediatric
103
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
autoimmune
neuropsychiatric disorders associated with
streptococcal
infection. PANDAS
are
distinct
from classic cases of OCD
because of their episodic
clinical course marked by
sudden symptom
exacerbation
linked to streptococcal infection, among
other unique features.
The
exacerbation of symptoms is correlated
with a rise in levels of antibodies
that the child produces
to
fight
the strep infection. Consequently,
researchers proposed that PANDAS
are caused by
antibodies
against
the strep infection that
also manage to attack the basal
ganglia region of the child's
brain
(Garvey
et al., 1998). In other words, the
strep infection triggers the
child's immune system to
develop
antibodies,
which, in turn, may attack the
child's brain, leading to
obsessive and compulsive
behaviors.
Under
this proposal, the strep
infection does not directly
induce the condition; rather, it
may do so
indirectly
by triggering antibody formation.
How the antibodies are so
damaging to a discrete region of
the
child's brain and how this
attack ignites OCD-like symptoms
are two of the fundamental
questions
guiding
research.
PSYCHOSOCIAL
INFLUENCES ON MENTAL HEALTH AND MENTAL
ILLNESS
Stressful
life events, affect (mood
and level of arousal), personality, and
gender are prominent
psychological
influences. Social influences
include parents, socioeconomic status,
racial, cultural, and
religious
background, and interpersonal
relationships.
Since
these psychosocial influences are
familiar to the general reader, detailed
description of each is
beyond
the scope of our study here.
Instead, we will summarize the sweeping
theories of individual
behavior
and personality that
inspired a vast body of psychosocial
research: psychodynamic theories,
behaviorism,
and social learning theories.
PSYCHODYNAMIC
THEORIES
Psychodynamic
theories of personality assert that
behavior is the product of underlying
conflicts over
which
people often have scant
awareness. Sigmund Freud
(18561939) was the towering
proponent of
psychoanalytic
theory, the first of the 20th-century
psychodynamic theories.
Many
of Freud's followers pioneered
their own psychodynamic theories,
but we will cover
only
psychoanalytic
theory. A brief discussion of
Freud's work contributes to a
historical perspective of
mental
health theory and treatment
approaches.
Freud's
theory of psychoanalysis holds two
major assumptions: (1) that
much of mental life
is
unconscious
(i.e., outside awareness), and
(2) that past experiences,
especially in early childhood,
shape
how
a person feels and behaves throughout
life (Brenner, 1978).
Freud's
structural model of personality
divides the personality into three
parts--the id, the ego, and the
superego.
The id is the unconscious part
that is the cauldron of raw
drives, such as for sex or
aggression.
The
ego, which has conscious and
unconscious elements, is the rational
and reasonable part
of
personality.
Its role is to maintain contact
with the outside world in
order to help keep the individual
in
touch
with society. As such, the ego
mediates between the conflicting
tendencies of the id and the
superego.
The
superego is a person's conscience that
develops early in life and is learned
from parents,
teachers,
and
others. Like the ego, the superego has
conscious and unconscious elements
(Brenner, 1978).
When
all three parts of the personality
are in dynamic equilibrium, the
individual is thought to be
mentally
healthy. However, according to
psychoanalytic theory, if the ego is
unable to mediate between
the
id and the superego, an imbalance would occur in the
form of psychological distress and
symptoms
of
mental disorders.
104
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Psychoanalytic
theory views symptoms as
important only in terms of expression of
underlying conflicts
between
the parts of personality. The
theory holds that the
conflicts must be understood by
the
individual
with the aid of the psychoanalyst who
would help the person unearth the
secrets of the
unconscious.
This was the basis for
psychoanalysis as a form of treatment.
BEHAVIORISM
AND SOCIAL LEARNING
THEORY
Behaviorism
(also called learning theory) posits
that personality is the sum of an
individual's observable
responses
to the outside world (Feldman,
1997). As charted by J. B. Watson and B. F. Skinner in
the
early
part of the 20th century,
behaviorism stands in opposition
with psychodynamic theories,
which
strive
to understand underlying
conflicts.
Behaviorism
rejects the existence of underlying
conflicts and an unconscious.
Rather, it focuses on
observable,
overt behaviors that are
learned from the environment (Kazdin,
1996, 1997). Its
application
to
treatment of mental problems is known as
behavior modification. Learning is
seen as behavior
change
molded
by experience. Learning is accomplished largely
through either classical or
operant
conditioning.
Classical
conditioning is grounded in the research
of Ivan Pavlov, a Russian
physiologist. It explains
why
some people react to
formerly neutral stimuli in
their environment, stimuli
that previously would
not
have elicited a reaction. Pavlov's
dogs, for example, learned to
salivate merely at the sound of
the
bell,
without any food in sight.
Originally, the sound of the bell
would not have elicited
salvation. But
by
repeatedly pairing the sight of the
food (which elicits
salvation on its own) with
the sound of the bell,
Pavlov
taught the dogs to salivate
just to the sound of the bell by
itself.
Operant
conditioning, a process described and
coined by B. F. Skinner, is a form of
learning in which a
voluntary
response is strengthened or attenuated, depending on
its association with positive or
negative
consequences
(Feldman, 1997). The
strengthening of responses occurs by
positive reinforcement,
such
as
food, pleasurable activities, and
attention from others. The
attenuation or discontinuation of
responses
occurs
by negative reinforcement in the form of
removal of a pleasurable stimulus. Thus,
human
behavior
is shaped in a trial and error
way through positive and
negative reinforcement, without
any
reference
to inner conflicts or perceptions. What
goes on inside the individual is
irrelevant, for
humans
are
equated with "black
boxes."
Mental
disorders represented maladaptive
behaviors that were learned.
They could be unlearned
through
behavior
modification (behavior therapy)
(Kazdin, 1996, 1997).
SOCIAL
LEARNING THEORY
The
movement beyond behaviorism was
spearheaded by Albert Bandura
(1969, 1977), the originator
of
social
learning theory (also known as
social cognitive theory). Social
learning theory has its
roots in
behaviorism,
but it departs in a significant
way. While acknowledging
classical and operant
conditioning,
social learning theory places
far greater emphasis on a different
type of learning,
particularly
observational learning. Observational
learning occurs through
selectively observing the
behavior
of another person, a model. When the
behavior of the model is rewarded,
children are more
likely
to imitate the behavior. For
example, a child who
observes another child receiving candy
for a
particular
behavior is more likely to carry
out similar
behaviors.
105
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Social
learning theory asserts that
people's cognitions--their views,
perceptions, and expectations
toward
their environment--affect what
they learn. Rather than
being passively conditioned by
the
environment,
as behaviorism proposed, humans take a
more active role in deciding
what to learn as a
result
of cognitive processing. Social
learning theory gave rise to
cognitive-behavioral therapy.
106
Table of Contents:
|
|||||