|
|||||
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
The
researcher can hypothesize that a
variable ratio pattern of
reinforcement will increase
behavior
more
rapidly than no reinforcement.
This hypothesis could then be
tested in relation to
clinically
relevant
behaviors, such as the development and
maintenance of a child's disruptive or
noncompliant
behavior.
Hypotheses
can also emerge directly
from the findings of previous
research. This includes
studies
carried
out by other researchers as
well as an investigator's own
previous work. Knowledge of
prior
research
is important to avoid pursuing the
answers to questions that have already
been resolved, to
learn
from the tribulations and mistakes of
others, and to draw on the findings of
previous studies as an
important
source of future hypotheses.
Keeping abreast of research in
psychology has become
a
daunting
task, however, as the field has
grown to include thousands of
active researchers
publishing
their
findings in hundreds of journals around
the world. Computerized literature
search programs such
as
PsychLit, PsychInfo, and MedLine have
been enormously helpful in
expediting the process of
bringing
oneself up to date on current research on
a topic. But these methods
are not a substitute
for
reading
broadly in many areas of
psychology to develop hypotheses
that reflect basic as well
as applied
research.
Some
of the best examples of programmatic
research in clinical psychology
involve the use of
basic
research
on a clinical disorder to form the
foundation for interventions to treat or
prevent the problem.
For
example, research on the factors that
place children at risk for the
development of aggressive
and
violent
behavior problems has led to the
development of interventions in childhood
to prevent the onset
of
these problems.
MEASURING
KEY VARIABLES
2.
SELECTION OF MEASURES
Once
a set of hypotheses has been
developed, the next challenge
for the researcher is to determine
how
to
measure the key variables,
or constructs, that are the focus of the
study. Measurement involves
assessment
of characteristics of people's thoughts emotions,
behavior, and physiology and
the,
environments
in which they function. A number of
difficult decisions must be made
with regard to the
measurement
of people and environments. First, the
aspects of the person or the
environment that are
most
central to the research goals and
hypotheses must be
determined.
A
researcher however, cannot, measure
everything that might be
relevant to the question at hand.
Measurement
of a large number of variables is
impractical, because participants in
research often cannot
or
will not invest the amount of
time that a researcher
desires. In general, researchers should
measure
only
those factors that are most
important to their
hypotheses.
Second,
specific methods must be
selected to measure the variables of
interest in the study. Assessment
methods
used in clinical research
include direct observations (e.g., observations of
parents and children
interacting
with each other);
self-reports by participants in the
research (e.g., self-reports of
symptoms
of
depression and anxiety); measures of
physiological reactivity and recovery
(e.g. heart rate variability,
skin
conductance); and performance on structured experimental
tasks (e.g., continuous
performance
tasks).
Each of the methods of measurement
has its inherent strengths
and weaknesses. For
example,
self-reports
from participants are
necessary to assess certain
aspects of thoughts and emotions
because
there
are aspects of private experience
that are not accessible
any other way.
On
the other hand, self-reports are subject
to certain types of problems, including
biases in the ways that
individuals
may want to present
them-selves to others, the inability to
accurately report on certain
aspects
of one's own thoughts and emotions, and
unwillingness to disclose certain types
of information.
Observational
methods are strong in terms of
their objectivity and ability to
measure behavior as it
occurs
in response to specific events or
conditions in the environment. Observations cannot be
used,
however,
to measure private thoughts and
internal emotional states.
One solution to the problems
81
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
inherent
in each form of measurement is to
use different types of measures in the
same study to
determine
the degree to which the findings converge
across different types of measurement as
opposed
to
findings that are unique to
one type of measure.
Third,
the researcher must determine if
tried-and-true measures of these
constructs have been
developed
and
used in previous research, or if
new measures need to be constructed to
pursue the goals of this
study.
Whenever possible, researchers use
measures that have established levels of
reliability and
validity
and (when appropriate) that have
normative data available on
populations that are similar
to the
participants
in the study. These factors provide a
degree of assurance that the
measure can be trusted -
that
the results are to some
degree consistent and
accurate. In some instances, a
researcher will want
to
measure
a variable for which an
adequate instrument is not
available. In these cases, the
researcher is
faced
with the task of developing and
validating a new measure in
order to carry out the
study. It is not
acceptable,
however, to simply employ a
new measure or technique for
the purposes of the study
without
paying careful attention to
establishing its reliability and
validity.
An
example of these difficult, decisions
can be found in research on the nature of
anxiety disorders.
Anxiety
can be measured at a number of different
levels, including the experience and emotions of
the
individual
(e.g., "I feel tense and anxious"),
observations made by others of overt
manifestations of
anxiety
(e.g., "I could see his
hands were shaking and I could
hear a trembling in his
voice"), and
measures
of physiological arousal (e.g., elevated heart rate,
blood pressure, skin conductance).
None of
these
approaches to measurement represents the
"right" way to assess
anxiety, and the issue is
clouded
by
the fact that the three approaches
often yield different
results.
For
example, some individuals
may experience high physiological arousal
but do not report
subjective
experiences
of fear or anxiety, and
conversely, some individuals
with very low levels of
arousal feel
very
anxious. The failure of
different types of measures to converge
(i.e., to provide the
same
information
on the variable that is being
measured) does not imply
that any one of the measures
is
invalid.
However; it presents the researcher
with a challenge in the interpretation of
the different sources
of
information.
3.
SELECTING A RESEARCH
DESIGN
Armed
with a clear set of hypotheses and
appropriate measures to assess the
important variables
under
investigation,
a clinical psychologist is prepared to design a
study. There are four
basic types of research
designs
(but many variations within
each) from which to choose:
descriptive
designs, correlational
designs,
experimental
designs, and single-case
designs. Generally, the
two main methods of
data
collection
are survey
method and
observational
method.
Moreover, all these designs
can be cross-
sectional
(one
point in time) or longitudinal
(over
the course of time). The choice of
which design and
which
data-collection method to use
depends on the nature of the question
being asked and on
ethical
and
practical limitations that
may constrain the research. No one type
of research design is inherently
superior
to others - each is simply
better suited to answer some questions
than others.
RESEARCH
METHODS
There
are two main categories of
research methods, both with
their own respective
sub-categories.
These
are
1.
SURVEY METHODS
2.
OBSERVATIONAL METHODS
1.
SURVEY RESEARCH
METHODS
Survey
methods are used to obtain
people's responses and opinions
regarding an issue or
problem.
Types
of survey include computerized on-line
surveys, telephonic surveys, voting
polls, personal
interviews,
use of questionnaires etc.
82
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
1.
THE PURPOSE OF SURVEY
RESEARCH METHODS
The
aim of survey research is to
measure certain attitudes
and/or behaviors of a population or a
sample.
The
attitudes might be opinions
about a political candidate or feelings
about certain issues or
practices.
2.
FOCUS
Survey
research focuses on naturally
occurring phenomena. Rather than
manipulating phenomena,
survey
research attempts to influence the
attitudes and behaviors it
measures as little as possible.
Most
often,
respondents are asked for
information.
3.
TYPES OF DATA
Survey
research is primarily quantitative,
but qualitative methods are
sometimes used, too.
4.
SAMPLING
Once
in a while, a researcher may be
able to gather data from all
members of a population. For
example,
if
you want to know what a
neighborhood thinks about a
local land use issue,
you may be able to
measure
all residents of the
neighborhood if it is not too
big. However, most of the
time, the population
is
so large that researchers
must sample only a part of
the population and make conclusions about
the
population
based on the sample. Because of
this, gaining a representative sample is
crucial in survey
research.
SOME
COMMON SAMPLING
STRATEGIES
SIMPLE
RANDOM SAMPLING
Members
of the population are drawn at random to
be in the sample. Each member of the
population has
an
equal chance of being in the
sample. Think of putting the
names of all the possible
survey
respondents
into a hat and drawing them out
one by one to build your
sample.
STRATIFIED
RANDOM SAMPLING
Strata
(sub-groups) are identified and
respondents selected at random from
within the relevant
strata.
For
example, if I want to know the
extent of certain health
behaviors among the students at my
college,
I
would identify the relevant dimensions.
These might be day or night
students (since these are
two
fairly
distinct sub-populations) and
major (since Letters, Arts,
and Sciences majors tend to be
different
from
Business majors). Thus, I would have 4 sub-groups:
day students in Business,
day students in
Letters,
Arts and Sciences, night
students in Business, and night
students in Letters, Arts and
Sciences.
Then,
I would randomly choose
respondents from within each
of these four groups. The
step of
stratifying
gives me a more targeted sampling
strategy.
PROPORTIONATE
SAMPLING
This
imposes the constraint that the
sample must reflect the same
proportions of sub-groups as is
found
in
the population. For example, I
could insist that my samples
have the same proportion of
traditional-
age
students (18-22) and
non-traditional students.
NON-PROBABILITY
SAMPLING
This
is a procedure in which the sampling strategy
does not give a representative
sample. Examples
include
convenience sampling, where the sample is
made up of those whom it is
most convenient to
survey,
say one's friends or people
who pass by a certain street
corner; self-selected sampling, in
which
83
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
the
respondents get to choose whether to be
included in the survey, such as
leaving questionnaires at a
table
in a public place; and snowball sampling,
in which previous respondents
recruit subsequent
respondents.
Note
that although these
non-probability sampling strategies do
not yield representative samples,
they
may
still be useful to researchers in
gaining a preliminary picture or as a
pilot project.
5.
POSSIBLE SOURCES OF BIAS IN
SURVEY RESEARCH
DEMAND
CHARACTERISTICS
Respondents
tend to say what they
think the researcher wants to
hear.
ACQUIESCENCE
Respondents
tend to say "yes" more
easily than "no."
REACTIVITY
Thinking
about the questions tends to change
respondents' opinions. For
example, you may not
have
thought
much about environmental
damage until a survey asks
for your opinions on
rainforest depletion.
RESPONSE
BIAS
Some
people tend to answer more
positively or in more extreme terms. If there is a
consistent tendency
for
one group to give more extreme responses
and a consistent tendency for another
group to give more
middle-of-the-road
responses, we might mistakenly conclude
they have different opinions. In
fact, we
may
only be observing a bias in
their response
tendencies.
2.
OBSERVATIONAL METHODS
The
most basic and pervasive of
all research methods is
observation. Experimental, case
study, and
naturalistic
approaches all involve
making observations of what someone is
doing or has done. Types
of
observational
methods include the
following:
a.
UNSYSTEMATIC OBSERVATION
Casual
observation does little by
itself to establish a strong base of
knowledge. However, it is
through
such
observation that we develop
hypotheses that can
eventually be subjected to test. For
example,
suppose
a clinician notes on several different
occasions that when a
patient struggles or has
difficulty
with
a specific item on an achievement test,
the effect seems to carry
over to the next item and
adversely
affect
performance.
This
observation leads the clinician to
formulate the hypothesis that performance
might be enhanced by
making
sure each failure item is
followed by an easy item on
which the patient will
likely succeed. This
should
help build the patient's confidence and
thus improve performance. To test
this prediction, the
clinician
might administer an experimental
version of the achievement test, in which
difficult items are
followed
by easy items. It would then be
relatively easy to develop a
study that would test
this
hypothesis
in a representative sample of
clients.
b.
NATURALISTIC OBSERVATION
Though
carried out in real-life settings,
naturalistic observation is more
systematic and rigorous. It is
neither
casual nor free-wheeling,
but carefully planned in
advance. However, there is no real
control
exerted
by the observer, who is pretty
much at the mercy of freely flowing
events. Frequently,
observations
are limited to a relatively
few individuals or situations. Thus, it
may be uncertain how
far
one
can generalize to other people or
other situations. It is also possible
that in the midst of observing
or
recording
responses, the observer may unwittingly
interfere with or influence the
events under study.
84
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
An
example of a study using the
naturalistic observation method
might be an investigation of
patient
behavior
in a psychiatric hospital. Perhaps a
particular unit in this
hospital is composed of patients
who
are
scheduled to undergo electroconvulsive
therapy (ECT) that day.
The clinician's job is to focus on
ten
patients
and observe each one
for 2 minutes every half-hour.
This observational study
might yield
interesting
data about the reactions of patients
prior to ECT. But with
only ten patients from
this
particular
hospital, can wide
generalizations be made? Are
these patients' reactions similar to
those in
other
hospitals or other units where the
overall atmosphere may be
very different? Or were the
patients
aware
of the observer's presence and
could they have altered
their customary reactions in order
to
somehow
impress him or her?
Investigators
committed to more rigorous experimental
methods sometimes condemn
naturalistic
observation
as too uncontrolled. But
this judgment may be too
harsh.
As
with unsystematic observation, this
method can serve as a rich
source of hypotheses that
can be
subjected
to careful scrutiny later.
Naturalistic observations do get investigators
closer to the real
phenomena
that interest them. Such observations
avoid the artificiality and contrived
nature of many
experimental
settings. For example, regardless of
their feelings about
psychodynamic theory,
most
acknowledge
that Freud's clinical
observation skills were extraordinary.
Freud used his own powers
of
observation
to construct one of the most influential
and sweeping theories in the history of
clinical
psychology.
It is important to recall that
Freud had available no objective
tests, no computer printouts,
and
no sophisticated experimental methods.
What he did possess was the
ability to observe, interpret,
and
generalize in an impressive fashion.
c.
CONTROLLED OBSERVATION
To
deal in part with the
foregoing criticisms of unsystematic and
naturalistic observation, some
clinical
investigators
employ controlled observation.
While the research may be
carried out in the field or
in
relatively
natural settings, the investigator exerts
some degree of control over
the events. Controlled
observation
has a long history in
clinical psychology. For
example, it is one thing to have patients
tell
clinicians
about their fears or check
off items on a questionnaire. However,
Bernstein, and Nietzel
(1973)
studied the nature of snake phobias by placing
study participants in the presence of
real snakes
and
then varying the distance between
participant and snake. This
controlled observation enabled
them
to
gain some real insight
into the nature of the participants' reactions.
Controlled observation can
also be
used
to assess communication patterns between
couples or spouses. Instead of relying on
distressed
couples'
self-reports of their communication
problems, researchers may choose to
actually observe
communications
styles in a controlled setting.
Specifically,
partners can be asked to
discuss and attempt to resolve a moderate-sized
relationship
problem
of their choosing (for example,
partner spends too much
money on unnecessary things)
while
researchers
observe or videotape the interaction
behind a one-way mirror.
Although not a substitute
for
naturalistic
observation of conflict and problem
solving in the home, researchers have
found this
controlled
observation method to be a useful and
cost-effective means of assessing
couples' interaction
patterns.
d.
CASE STUDIES
The
case study method involves the
intensive study of a client or
patient who is in treatment. Under
the
heading
of case studies we include
material from interviews,
test responses, and treatment
accounts.
Such
material might also include
biographical and autobiographical data, letters,
diaries, life-course
information,
medical histories, and so on.
Case studies, then, involve
the intensive study and
description
of
one person. Such studies have
long been prominent in the
study of abnormal behavior and in
the
description
of treatment methods. Their great value
resides in their richness as
potential sources of
understanding
and as hypothesis generators. They can
serve as excellent preludes to
scientific
investigation.
85
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
Nothing
will ever likely supplant
the case study as a way of
helping clinicians to under-stand
that
unique
patient who sits there
before them. As Allport (1961) so
compellingly argued, individuals
must
be
studied individually. Case
studies have been especially
useful for (1) providing
descriptions of rare or
unusual
phenomena or novel, distinctive
methods of interviewing, assessing, or
treating patients;
(2)
disconfirming
"universally" known or accepted
information; and (3) generating testable
hypotheses.
There
is, of course, a downside to case
study methods. For example,
it is difficult to use individual
cases
to
develop universal laws or behavioral
principles that apply to
everyone. Likewise, one case
study
cannot
lead to cause-effect conclusions because
clinicians are not able to
control important
variables
that
have operated in that case. For
example, one patient may
benefit enormously from
psychodynamic
therapy
for reasons that have less
to do with the therapy method than
with the personality
characteristics
of
that patient. Only
subsequent controlled research
can pin down the exact
causes of, or factors
influencing
change.
CROSS-SECTIONAL
VERSUS LONGITUDINAL
APPROACHES
Another
way of classifying research
studies is by considering whether the
studies are cross-sectional
or
longitudinal
in nature. A cross-sectional design is one that
evaluates or compares individuals,
perhaps of
different
age groups, at the same point in
time. A longitudinal design follows the
same subjects over
time.
The
basic format of these two
approaches is shown in the
following figure. In this example,
row
(a)
illustrates the longitudinal design
and column (b) the
cross-sectional design.
Cross-sectional
approaches are correlational,
because the investigator cannot
manipulate age nor
can
participants
be assigned to different age groups.
Because there are different
participants in each
age
group,
we cannot assume that the outcome of the
study reflects age changes; it
only reflects differences
among
the age groups employed. These
differences could be due to the
eras in which participants
were
raised
rather than age by itself.
For example, a group of
65-year-olds might show up as more frugal
than
a
group of 35-year-olds. Does
this mean that advancing
age promotes frugality? Perhaps.
But it might
simply
reflect the historical circumstance
that the 65-year-olds were raised
during the Great Depression
when
money was very hard to
come by.
Longitudinal
studies are those in which
we collect data on the same
people over time. Such
designs
allow
us to gain insight into how
behavior or mental processes
change with age. In the
interpretive
sense,
longitudinal studies enable investigators
to better speculate about
time-order relationships among
factors
that vary together. They
also help eliminate the
third variable problem that
so often arises in
correlational
studies. For example,
suppose we know that states
of depression come and go over
the
years.
If depression is responsible for the
correlation between significant weight
loss and decreased
self-
confidence,
then both weight loss and
decline in self-confidence should
vary along with
depressive
states.
There
are, of course many
variations in cross-sectional and
longitudinal deigns. In the case
of
longitudinal
studies, however, the main problems
are practical ones. Such
studies are costly to carry
out,
and
they require great patience and
continuity or leadership in the research
program. Sometimes,
too,
researchers
must live with design
mistakes made years earlier
or put up with outmoded
research and
assessment
methods. Because longitudinal
research is expensive in both
time and money, it is not
used
as
often as it should be. For
these reasons, research in the
developmental aspects or
psychopathology
has
long suffered. Still, it is to be hoped
that there will be a return to
those strategies that deal
with
psychopathology,
treatment, or personality over extended periods of
time, using a variety of
measures.
Too
often, we have been captives of a
cross-sectional methodology that
sometimes seems to focus
exclusively
on 50-minute experiments. Such strategies
have promoted a "snapshot" view of
human
behavior
and personality that has done
little to help us understand the
coherence and organization of
human
behavior.
86
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
4.
SELECTING A SAMPLE
Who
should participate in a study
once it has been designed?
Selection and recruitment of a sample
is
important
to the ultimate generalizability
(external validity) of the research
findings. If the sample is
not
a
representative sample of the larger
population from which it is
drawn, the results of the study
may be
biased
or influenced by the characteristics of the
sample. For example, a
sample may differ from
the
general
population in terms of demographic
characteristics, such as sex, age,
education level,
income,
and
ethnic or racial background.
These characteristics may
influence the findings of the study,
because
the
results may differ as a
function of one or more of these
characteristics.
In
research with clinical
samples, it is additionally important to
determine the extent to which
the
sample
is representative of the clinical population to
which the results are to be
generalized.
5.
TESTING HYPOTHESES
STATISTICAL
VERSUS PRACTICAL
SIGNIFICANCE
Once
a study has been completed and the
data are in hand, psychologists rely on
the use of inferential
statistics
to evaluate the degree to which the
null hypothesis has been rejected.
The specific type of
statistical
procedure, which is used, depends on the
research design that was
employed. After a statistic
(such
as a correlation coefficient) has
been calculated, it can be
determined whether the
obtained
number
is significant.
Traditionally,
if it is found that the obtained
value (or a more extreme value)
could be expected to occur
by
chance alone less than 5
times out of 100, it is
deemed statistically significant.
Such an obtained
value
is said to be significant at the .05
Ievel, usually written as p <
.05. The larger the
statistical value,
the
more likely it is to be significant. But
when large numbers of
participants are involved,
even
relatively
small statistical values can be
significant. With 180
participants, a correlation of .19
will be
significant;
when only 30 participants
are involved, a correlation of
.30 would fail to be
significant.
Therefore,
it is important to distinguish between
statistical significance and
practical significance
when
interpreting
statistical results. The
correlation of .19 may be
significant, but the magnitude of
the
relationship
is still quite modest. For
example, it might be true
that in a study involving
5000 second-
year
graduate students in clinical psychology
across the nation, there is a correlation
of .15 between
their
GRE scores and faculty
ratings of academic competence.
Even though the relationship is
not a
chance
one, the actual importance is rather
small. Most of the variance in
faculty ratings is due to
factors
other
than GRE scores.
In
some cases, a correlation of
.15 may be judged important,
but in many instances, it is
not. At the
same
time, we should remember
that accepting significance levels of
.05 as non-chance represents
a
kind
of scientific tradition - it is not
sacred. Other information
may persuade us, in certain
cases such as
clinical
settings that significance levels of
.07 or .09, for example,
should be taken
seriously.
In
other words, clinical researchers
need to look beyond the
statistical significance of the findings
to
understand
the statistic's meaning for the
people and problems that are the focus of
clinical research.
6.
INTERPRETING AND DISSEMINATING
FINDINGS
The
final step in the research
process is to place the meaning or
implications of a study in a
broader
context.
What are the implications of the
findings for understanding the nature,
causes, and course of
psychological
problems? What are the implications
for the prevention or treatment of
psychopathology?
What
do the results mean for
establishing public policy
related to mental
health?
87
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
A
first step in the process of sharing the
results of research is to submit them to
review for publication
in
peer-reviewed
professional journals. Articles
that are published in
peer-reviewed scientific journals
have
been
evaluated by other researchers and
experts in the field prior to
publication. The review
process
ensures
that the work that is
published meets certain
accepted criteria for
scientific quality. The
most
rigorous
journals in clinical psychology
include Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology, Behavior Therapy
etc.
The
findings of clinical research
are not designed solely for
advancing the science of
clinical
psychology,
however. Clinical research is
also designed to improve the conditions
of people with
psychological
problems. Therefore, researchers have an
obligation to translate their findings
into
information
that can be used for the
general good. Research results
should be communicated to the
public,
to practicing psychologists, and to officials
who formulate mental health
policies and allocate
money
for mental health programs.
Practicing psychologists and their
clients as well as policy
makers
are
hungry for information that
will help them understand the nature,
causes, and treatment of
psychological
problems.
SUMMARY
AND CONCLUSIONS
The
foundation of clinical psychology
lies in the research that
has been generated on the nature
and
causes
of psychopathology, the development of
measures of personality and behavior, and
the
evaluation
of the effects of psychotherapy and other forms of
intervention to relieve or
prevent
psychological
distress. The research
process follows a series of
steps that include the
generation of
hypotheses,
the choice of measures, the selection of a research
design, the identification of a sample,
the
testing
of the hypothesis, and the interpretation and
dissemination of findings. Clinical
psychologists use
several
different types of research designs,
including single-case designs,
descriptive methods,
correlational
designs, and experimental methods.
Using both correlational and
experimental methods to
conduct
studies in the laboratory and in the
natural environment, clinical
psychologists have made
significant
contributions to the scientific study of
human behavior.
RESEARCH
ETHICS
Just
like clinical practice and
all other areas of
psychology, psychological research,
too, involves
important
ethical considerations. Like patients,
research participants have rights, and
investigators have
responsibilities
to them.
In
1992, the American Psychological
Association published an expanded and
updated set of ethical
standards
for research with human
participants (APA, 1992). We
offer only a brief overview
here. These
standards
require that
investigators:
1.
Plan
research according to recognized
standards of scientific competence
and ethical
principles
2.
Implement
safeguards for the welfare of
participants, others that
may be affected by the
research,
and animal subjects
3.
Retain
responsibility for ensuring
ethical practices in
research
4
Comply
with pertinent federal and
state law and
regulations
5.
Gain
appropriate approval from
host institutions or organizations
before conducting
research
6.
Establish
clear and fair agreements with
their research participants so
that the rights and
obligations
of each party are
clarified
88
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
7.
Obtain
the informed consent of research
participants, using language that is
easily
understandable
to them, and document their
consent
8.
Take
great care, in offering inducements for
research participation, that the nature
of the
compensation
(such as professional services) is
made clear and that
financial or other types of
inducements
are not be so excessive as to
coerce participation
9.
Use
deception as part of their
procedures only when it is
not possible to use alternative
methods
10.
Protect
participants from any mental
and physical discomfort, harm, and danger
that might arise
during
the research
11.
Inform
research participants of the anticipated
use of the data and of the possibility of
sharing
the
data with other
investigators or any unanticipated
future uses
12.
Minimize
the invasiveness of research
procedures
13.
Provide
participants with information at the
close of the research to erase
any misconceptions
that
may have arisen
14.
Treat
animal subjects humanely and
in accordance with federal,
state, and local laws, as
well as
with
professional standards
Several
of these points require
further comment.
INFORMED
CONSENT
Good
ethical practice as well as legal
requirements demand that participants
give their formal
informed
consent
(usually in writing) prior to
their participation in research.
Researchers inform the
participants
of
any risks, discomforts, or limitations on
confidentiality. Further, researchers
inform the participants
of
any compensation for their
participation. In the process, the
researcher agrees to guarantee
the
participant's
privacy, safety, and freedom
to withdraw. Unless participants know the
general purpose of
the
research and the procedures that
will be used, they cannot
fully exercise their
rights.
CONFIDENTIALITY
Participant's
individual data and responses
should be confidential and guarded from
public scrutiny.
Instead
of names, code numbers are
typically used to protect
anonymity. While the results of
the
research
are usually open to the public,
they are presented in such a
way that no one can identify
a
specific
participant's data. Finally, clinical
psychologists must obtain consent
before disclosing any
confidential
or personally identifiable information in
the psychologist's writings, lectures, or
presentations
in any other public media
(such as a television
interview).
DECEPTION
Sometimes,
the purpose of the research or the
meaning of a participant's responses is
withheld. Such
deception
should be used only when the
research is important and there is no
alternative to the deception
(in
other words, when veridical
information would compromise
participants' data). Deception
should
never
be used lightly. When it is
used, extreme care must be
taken that participants do
not leave the
research
setting feeling exploited or
disillusioned. It is important that
careful debriefing be
undertaken
so
that participants are told
exactly why the deception
was necessary. We do not
want participants'
levels
of interpersonal trust to be shaken.
Clearly, it is very important
how we obtain informed
consent
when
deception is involved.
89
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
An
example of the need for
deception in a study might be an
experiment in which it is predicted
that the
viewing
of gun magazines (or other
materials associated with potential
violence) will lead to
increased
scores
on a questionnaire measuring hostility. All
participants are told that
the experiment is one
focusing
on short-term memory, and they will be
completing a memory task on
two occasions
separated
by
a 15-minute waiting period
during which they will be
reading magazine articles. All
participants first
complete
baseline measures (including the
hostility questionnaire). Next,
all participants complete a
computer-administered
memory task. During the waiting
period, the experimental group is
told to read
selections
from a gun magazine that is
made available in the lab; the
control group is told to
read
selections
from a nature magazine (neutral
with regard to violent imagery). All
participants later
complete
the computer-administered memory task
again. Finally, all
participants complete the battery
of
self-report
instruments a second time.
We
are not so much interested in the
viability of this hypothesis as we are in
the need for some
deception
in the experiment. As you can
see, to tell the participants the
real purpose of the
experiment
would
likely influence their
responses to the questionnaires (especially to the one
measuring hostility).
Therefore,
the investigator might need to
introduce the experiment as one
that is focusing on
short-term
memory.
DEBRIEFING
Because
participants have a right to know
why researchers are interested in
studying their behavior,
a
debriefing
at the end of the research is mandatory. It
should be explained to participants
why the
research
is being carried out, why it
is important, and what the
results have been. In some
cases, it is not
possible
to discuss results because the
research is still in progress.
But subjects can be told
what kinds of
results
are expected and that they
may return at a later date
for a complete briefing if they
wish.
FRAUDULENT
DATA
It
hardly seems necessary to
mention that investigators
are under the strictest standards of
honesty in
reporting
their data. Under no
circumstances may they alter
obtained data in any way. To
do so can
bring
charges of fraud and create
enormous legal, professional, and
ethical problems for the
investigator.
Although the frequency of fraud in
psychological research has so
far been minimal, we
must
be on guard. There is no quicker
way to lose the trust of the public
than through
fraudulent
practices.
90
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
LESSON
12
THE
CONCEPT OF ABNORMAL BEHAVIOR & MENTAL ILLNESS
Clinical
psychology is usually thought of as an
applied field. Clinicians attempt to
apply empirically
supported
psychological principles to problems of adjustment and
abnormal behavior. Typically
this
involves
finding successful ways of changing the
behavior, thoughts, and feelings of
clients. In this way,
clinical
psychologists lessen their clients'
maladjustment or dysfunction or increase
their levels of
adjustment.
Before
clinicians can formulate and
administer interventions, however,
they must first assess
their
clients'
symptoms of psychopathology and
levels of maladjustment. Interestingly,
the precise definitions
of
these and related terms can
be elusive. Further, the manner in
which the terms are applied
to clients is
sometimes
quite unsystematic.
Clinical
psychology has moved beyond
the primitive views that
defined mental illness as
possession by
demons
or spirits. Maladjustment is no longer
considered a state of sin. The
eighteenth and nineteenth
centuries
ushered in the notion that
"insane" individuals are
sick and require humane
treatment. Even
then,
however, mental health
practices could be bizarre, to
say the least. Clearly,
clinical psychologists'
contemporary
views are considerably more sophisticated
than those of their forebears.
Yet many view
current
treatments such as electroconvulsive
therapy (ECT) with some
skepticism and concern. Still
others
may see the popularity of
treatments using psychotropic medications
(such as antipsychotic,
antidepressant,
anti-manic, or anti-anxiety medications) as
less than
enlightened.
Finally,
many forms of "psychological treatment"
(for example, primal scream
therapy, age
regression
therapy)
are questionable at best. All of
these treatment approaches and
views are linked to the
ways
clinical
psychologists decide who needs
assessment, treatment, or intervention, as
well as the rationale
for
providing these services.
These judgments are
influenced by the labels or diagnoses
often applied to
people.
WHAT
IS ABNORMAL BEHAVIOR?
Ask
ten different people for a
definition of abnormal behavior and
you may get ten different
answers.
Some
of the reasons that abnormal
behavior is so difficult to define
are (1) no single
descriptive feature
is
shared by all forms of abnormal
behavior, and no one criterion
for "abnormality" is sufficient; and
(2)
no
discrete boundary exists between normal
and abnormal behavior. Many myths
about abnormal
behavior
survive and flourish even in this
age of enlightenment. For
example, many individuals
still
equate
abnormal behavior with (1)
bizarre behavior, (2)
dangerous behavior, or (3) shameful
behavior.
In
this section, we will examine in
some detail three proposed definitions of
abnormal behavior:
(1)
conformity
to norms, (2) the experience of
subjective distress, and (3)
disability or dysfunction. We
will
discuss
the pros and cons of each
definition. Although each of
these three definitions highlights
an
important
part of our; understanding of
abnormal behavior; each
definition, by itself, is
incomplete.
A.
CONFORMITY TO NORMS: STATISTICAL
INFREQUENCY OR VIOLATION OF
SOCIAL
NORMS
When
a person's behavior tends to conform to
prevailing social norms or
when this particular
behavior
is
frequently observed in other people, the
individual is not likely to
come to the attention of
mental
health
professionals. However, when a person's
behavior becomes patently
deviant, outrageous, or
otherwise
nonconforming, then he or she is more
likely to be categorized as "abnormal."
Let us consider
some
examples.
91
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
THE
CASE OF BILLY A
Billy
is now in the second grade. He is of average
height and weight and manifests no
physical
problems.
He is somewhat aggressive and tends to
bully children smaller than
himself. His birth. was
a
normal
one, and although he was a bit
slow in learning to walk and
talk, the deficit was not
marked. The
first
grade was difficult for
Billy, and his progress
was slow. By the end of the school
year, he was
considerably
behind the rest of the class.
However, the school officials decided to
pass him anyway.
They
reasoned that he was merely
a bit slow in maturing and
would "come around" shortly.
They noted
that
his status as an only child,
a pair of doting parents, a short
attention span, and
aggressiveness were
all
factors that combined to produce his
poor school performance.
At
the beginning of the second grade, Billy
was administered a routine achievement
test, on which he
did
very poorly. As a matter of school
policy, he was referred to the school
psychologist for
individual
testing
and evaluation. Based on the results of
the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, a
Draw-a-Person
Test,
school records, and a social history
taken from the parents, the
psychologist concluded that
Billy
suffered
from mental retardation. His
IQ was 64 on the Stanford-Binet and
was estimated to be 61 based
on
the Draw-a-Person Test. Further, a social
maturity index derived from
parental reports of his social
behavior
was quite low.
THE
CASE OF MARTHA L
Martha
seemed to have a normal childhood.
She made adequate progress
in school and caused few
problems
for her teachers or parents.
Although she never made
friends easily, she could
not be described
as
withdrawn. Her medical
history was negative. When
Martha entered high school, changes
began. She
combed
her hair in a very severe,
plain style. She chose
clothing that was quite ill
fitting and almost like
that
worn 50 years ago. She wore
neither makeup nor jewelry of
any kind. Where before she
would have
been
hard to distinguish from the
other girls in her class,
she now easily stood
out.
Martha's
schoolwork began to slip.
She spent hours alone in her
room reading the Bible. She
also began
slipping
notes to other girls that
commented on their immorality
when she observed them holding
hands
with
boys, giggling, dancing; and so on.
She attended religious services
constantly; sometimes on
Sundays
she went to services at five
or six separate churches.
She fasted frequently and
decorated her
walls
at home with countless pictures of
Christ, religious quotations; and
crucifixes.
When
Martha finally told her
parents that she was
going to join an obscure
religious sect and
travel
about
the country (in a state of
poverty) to bring Christ's message to the
country, they became
concerned
and
took her to a psychiatrist. Shortly
afterward, she was
hospitalized. Her diagnosis varied,
but it
included
such terms as schizophrenia, paranoid
type; schizoid personality; and
schizophrenia,
undifferentiated
type.
Both
of these cases are examples
of individuals commonly seen by
clinical psychologists for
evaluation
or
treatment. The feature that
immediately characterizes both
cases is that Billy's and
Martha's behaviors
violate
norms. Billy may be considered
abnormal because his IQ and school
performance depart
considerably
from the mean. This aspect
of deviance from the norm is very clear
in Billy's case,
because
it
can be described statistically and
with numbers. Once this
numerical categorization is
accomplished,
Billy's
assignment to the deviant category is
assured. Martha also came to
people's attention because
she
is
different. Her clothes, appearance, and
interests do not conform to the
norms typical of females in
her
culture.
ADVANTAGES
OF THIS DEFINITION
The
definition of abnormality in terms of
statistical infrequency or violation of
social norms is
attractive
for
at least two reasons.
1.
Cutoff Points: The
statistical infrequency approach is
appealing because it establishes
cutoff points
that
are quantitative in nature. If the cutoff
point on a scale is 80 and individual
scores a 75, the
decision
to
label that individual's
behavior as abnormal is relatively
straightforward. This principle of
statistical
deviance
is frequently used in the interpretation
of psychological test scores.
The test authors designate
a
cutoff point in the test
manual often based on
statistical deviance from the mean
score obtained by a
92
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
"normal"
sample of test-takers, and scores at or
beyond the cutoff are considered
"clinically significant"
(that
is, abnormal or deviant).
2.
Intuitive Appeal: It may
seem obvious to us that
those behaviors we ourselves consider
abnormal
would
be evaluated similarly by others.
The struggle to define
exactly what abnormal
behavior is does
not
tend to bother us because, as a
Supreme Court justice once
said about pornography, we
believe that
we
know it when we see
lt.
PROBLEMS
WITH THIS DEFINITION
Conformity
criteria seem to play a subtle
yet important role in our
judgments of others. However,
although
we must systematically seek the
determinants of the individual's nonconformity or
deviance,
should
resist the reflexive tendency to categorize
every nonconformist behavior as evidence
of mental
health
problems. Conformity criteria, in fact,
have a number of problems.
1.
Choice of Cutoff Points: Conformity
oriented definitions are
limited by the difficulty of
establishing
agreed-upon
cutoff points. As noted
previously, a cutoff is very
easy to use once it is
established.
However,
very few guidelines are
available for choosing the cutoff
point. For example, in the
case of
Billy,
is there some thing magical
about an of 64?
Traditional
practice sets the cutoff point at
70. Get an IQ below 70 and
you may be diagnosed
with
mental
retardation. But is a score of 69
all that different from a
score of 72? Rationally justifying
such
arbitrary
IQ cutoff points is difficult.
This problem is equally
salient in Martha's case.
Are five
crucifixes
on the wall too many? Is attendance at
three church services per week
acceptable?
2.
The Number of Deviations: Another
difficulty with nonconformity
standards is the number of
behaviors
that one must evidence in order to
earn the label "deviant." In
Martha's case, was it just
the
crucifixes,
or was it the total behavioral
configuration-crucifixes, clothes, makeup, withdrawal,
fasting,
and
so on? Had Martha manifested only three
categories of unusual behavior, would we
still classify her
as
deviant?
3.
Cultural Relativity: Martha's
case, in particular, illustrates an
additional point. Her
behavior was not
deviant
in some absolute sense. Had
she been a member of an
exceptionally religious family
that
subscribed
to radical religious beliefs
and practices, she might
never have been classified
as
maladjusted.
In short, what is deviant for
one group is not necessarily so
for another. Thus, the notion
of
cultural
relativity is important. Likewise,
judgments can vary, depending on
whether family, school
authorities
or peers are making them.
Such variability may
contribute to considerable
diagnostic
unreliability,
because even clinicians' judgments may be
relative to those of the group or groups
to
which
they belong.
Two
other points about cultural
relativity are also
relevant. First, carrying
cultural relativity notions
to
the
extreme can place nearly every reference
group beyond reproach. Cultures
can be reduced to
subcultures
and subcultures to mini-cultures. If we are
not careful, this reduction
process can result in
our
judging nearly every
behavior as healthy. Second, the
elevation of conformity to a position
of
preeminence
can be alarming. One is
reminded that so-called nonconformists
have made some of the
most
beneficial social contributions. It can
also become very easy to
remove those whose different
or
unusual
behavior bothers society. Some
years ago in Russia, political dissidents
were often placed in
mental
hospitals. In America, it sometimes
happens that 70-year-old
Uncle Arthur's family is
successful
in
hospitalizing him largely to
obtain his power of
attorney. His deviation is
that, at age 70, he
is
spending
too much of the money that
will eventually be inherited by the
family. Finally, if all
these
points
are not enough, excessive
conformity has itself
sometimes been the basis for
judging persons
abnormal.
93
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
B.
SUBJECTIVE DISTRESS
We
now shift the focus from the perceptions
of the observer to the perceptions of the
affected
individual.
Here the basic data are
not observable deviations of behavior,
but the subjective feelings
and
sense
of well-being of the individual. Whether
a person feels happy or sad,
tranquil or troubled,
and
fulfilled
or barren are the crucial considerations. If the
person is anxiety-ridden, then he or
she is
maladjusted,
regard-less of whether the anxiety
seems to produce overt behaviors
that are deviant in
some
way.
THE
CASE OF CYNTHIA S
Cynthia
has been married for 23
years. Her husband is a
highly successful civil
engineer. They have
two
children,
one in high school and the other in
college. There is nothing in
Cynthia's history to
suggest
psychological
problems. She is above average in
intelligence, and she completed two
years of college
before
marrying. Her friends all
characterize her as devoted to her
family. Of all her features, those
that
seem
to describe her best include her strong
sense of responsibility and a capacity to
get things done.
She
has always been a "coper."
She can continue to function
effectively despite a great deal of
personal
stress
and anxiety. She is a warm
person, yet not one to wear her
feelings or her troubles on her
sleeve.
She
recently enrolled in a night
course at the local community
college. In that course, the
students were
asked
to write an "existential" account of
their innermost selves. The
psychologist who taught the
course
was
surprised to find the following excerpts
in Cynthia's account:
"In
the morning, I often feel as if I cannot
make it through the day. I frequently
experience headaches
and
feel that I am getting sick.
I am terribly frightened when I have to
meet new people or serve as
a
hostess
at a party. At times I feel a
tremendous sense of sadness;
whether this is because of my
lack of
personal
identity, I don't
know."
What
surprised the instructor was that none of
these expressed feelings were apparent
from Cynthia's
overt
behavior she appeared
confident, reasonably assertive, competent, in
good spirits, and
outgoing.
THE
CASE OF ROBERT G
In
the course of a routine screening
report for a promotion,
Robert was interviewed by the
personnel
analyst
in the accounting company for which he
worked. A number of Robert's peers in the
office were
also
questioned about him. In the course of
these interviews, several things were
established.
Robert
was a very self-confident
person. He seemed very sure
of his goals and what he needed to do
to
achieve
them. Although hardly a happy-go-lucky
person, he was certainly content
with his progress so
far.
He never expressed the anxieties
and uncertainty that seemed
typical of so many of his
peers. There
was
nothing to suggest any
internal distress. Even his
enemies conceded that Robert
really "had it
together".
These
enemies began to be quite
visible as the screening process moved
along. Not many people in
the
office
liked Robert. He tended to use
people and was not above
stepping on them now and then to
keep
his
career moving. He was
usually inconsiderate and frequently
downright cruel. He was
particularly
insensitive
to those below him. He loved
ethnic humor and seemed to
revel in his prejudices
toward
minority
groups and those women who
intruded into a "man's world."
Even at home, his wife and
son
could
have reported that they were
kept in a constant turmoil
because of his insensitive
demands for
their
attention and
services.
Cynthia
and Robert are obviously two
very different kinds of
people. Cynthia's behavior is, in a
sense,
quite
conforming. Her ability to
cope would be cause for
admiration by many. Yet she
is unhappy and
conflicted,
and she experiences much
anxiety. A clinical psychologist
might not be surprised if
she
turned
up in the consulting room. Her
friends, however, would
likely be shocked were they to
learn that
she
had sought psychological help.
94
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
In
contrast, many of Robert's friends,
associates, and family
members would be gratified if he were
to
seek
help, since most of them have, at one
time or another, described
him as sick. But Robert is
not at
odds
with himself. He sees
nothing wrong with himself,
and he would probably react
negatively to any
suggestion
that he should seek therapy.
Furthermore, his lack of
motivation for therapy would
probably
make
it an unprofitable venture.
ADVANTAGES
OF THIS DEFINITION
Defining
abnormal behavior in terms of
subjective distress has some
appeal. It seems reasonable to
expect
that individuals can assess
whether they are
experiencing emotional or behavioral
problems and
can
share this information when
asked to do so. Indeed, many
methods of clinical assessment
(for
example,
self-report inventories, clinical
interviews) assume that the
respondent is aware of his or her
internal
state and will respond to
inquiries about personal distress in an
honest manner. In some ways,
this
relieves the clinician of the burden of
making an absolute judgment as to the
respondent's degree of
maladjustment.
PROBLEMS
WITH THIS DEFINITION
The
question is whether Cynthia,
Robert, or both are maladjusted.
The judgment will depend
upon one's
criteria
or values. From a strict standpoint of
subjective report, Cynthia
qualifies but Robert does
not.
This
example suggests that
labeling someone maladjusted is not
very meaningful unless the
basis for the
judgment
is specified and the behavioral
manifestations are
stated.
Not
everyone whom we consider to be
"disordered" reports subjective distress.
For example,
clinicians
sometimes
encounter individuals who may have
little contact with reality
yet profess inner
tranquility.
Nonetheless,
these individuals are
institutionalized. Such examples
remind us that subjective
reports
must
yield at times to other
criteria.
Another
problem concerns the amount of subjective
distress necessary to be considered
abnormal. All of
us
become aware of our own
anxieties from time to time,
so the total absence of such
feelings cannot be
the
sole criterion of adjustment. How
much anxiety is allowed, and
for how long, before we
acquire a
label?
Many would assert that the
very fact of being alive and
in an environment that can
never wholly
satisfy
us will inevitably bring
anxieties. Thus, as in the case of other
criteria, using
phenomenological
reports
is subject to limitations. There is a
certain charm to the idea
that if we want to know
whether a
person
is maladjusted, we should ask that
person, but there are
obvious pitfalls in doing
so.
C.
DISABILITY OR DYSFUNCTION
A
third definition of abnormal
behavior invokes the concept of
disability or dysfunction. For
behavior to
be
considered abnormal, it must create
some degree of social (interpersonal) or
occupational problems
for
the individual. Dysfunction in these
two spheres is often quite
apparent to both the individual and
the
clinician.
For example, a lack of
friendships or of relationships because
of a lack of interpersonal
contact
would be considered indicative of social
dysfunction, whereas the loss of
one's job because of
emotional
problems (such as depression) would
suggest occupational
dysfunction.
THE
CASE OF RICHARD Z
Richard
was convinced by his wife to
consult with a clinical
psychologist. Previous contacts
with
psychiatrists
had on one occasion resulted in a diagnosis of
"hypochondriacal neurosis," and on
another,
a
diagnosis of "passive aggressive personality."
Richard has not worked in
several years, even though he
has
a bachelor's degree in library science.
He claims that he is unable to find
employment because of
his
health.
He reports a variety of physical
symptoms, including dizziness,
breathlessness, weakness,
and
"funny"
sensations in the abdominal area.
Making the rounds from physician to
physician has enabled
him
to build an impressive stock of pills
that he takes incessantly.
None of his physicians, however,
has
been
able to find anything
physically wrong with
him.
95
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
As
a child, Richard was the
apple of his mother's eye.
She doted on him, praised
him constantly, and
generally
reinforced the notion that he
was someone special. His
father disappeared about 18
months
after
Richard was born. His
mother died six years ago,
and he married shortly after
that. Since then, his
wife
has supported both of them, thus
enabling him to finish
college. Only recently has
she begun to
accept
the fact that something may be
wrong with Richard.
THE
CASE OF PHYLLIS H
Phyllis
is a college student. She is in her sixth
year of undergraduate study but
has not yet obtained
a
degree.
She has changed majors at
least four times and has
also had to withdraw from school on
four
occasions.
Her
withdrawals from school have been
associated with her drug
habit. In two instances, her
family
placed
her in a mental hospital; on two
other occasions, she served
short jail sentences
following
convictions
on shoplifting charges. From
time to time, Phyllis
engages in minor crimes to support
her
drug
habit. Usually she can
secure the money from her
parents, who seem to have an
uncanny knack for
accepting
her outrageous justifications. She has
been diagnosed with "antisocial
personality disorder"
and
with "drug dependence
(cocaine)."
According
to the disability/dysfunction definition,
both of these cases would
suggest the presence of
abnormal
behavior. Richard is completely dependent
on his wife (social
dysfunction), and this,
coupled
with
his litany of somatic
complaints and his inability to
cope with stress, has
left him unemployed
(occupational
dysfunction). Phyllis's drug
habit has interfered with
her occupational (in this
case,
school)
functioning.
ADVANTAGES
OF THIS DEFINITION
Perhaps
the greatest advantage to adopting this
definition of abnormal behavior is
that relatively
little
inference
is required. Problems in both the social
and occupational sphere often
prompt individuals to
seek
out treatment. It is often the case
that individuals come to
realize the extent of their
emotional
problems
when these problems affect
their family or social relationships as
well as significantly
affect
their
performance at either work or
school.
PROBLEMS
WITH THIS DEFINITION
Who
should establish the standards for
social or occupational dysfunction, the
patient, the therapist,
friends,
or the employer? In some ways, judgments
regarding both social and
occupational functioning
are
relative-not absolute-and involve a
value-oriented standard. Although
most of us may agree
that
having
relationships and contributing to society
as an employee or student are valuable
characteristics, it
is
harder to agree on what specifically
constitutes an adequate level of
functioning in these spheres.
In
short,
achieving individual's social
relationships and contributions as a
worker or student may be
difficult.
Recognizing this problem,
psychopathologists have developed self-report
inventories and
special
interviews to assess social and
occupational functioning in a systematic
and reliable way.
To
summarize, several criteria are
used to define abnormal
behavior. Each criterion has
its advantages
and
disadvantages, and no one criteria can be
used as a gold standard.
Some subjectivity is involved
in
applying
any of these criteria. As
Phares has stated,
The
inevitable conclusion is that a
definition of abnormality (maladjustment,
pathology, etc.) is possible
only
with reference to a set of value
judgments. To characterize someone as
abnormal is to assert that
he
needs
treatment. In short, someone has decided
that the patient needs help
in changing his behaviors-a
relative,
a court, or perhaps the patient
himself. Once someone
decides that the patient
needs treatment,
then
our psychiatrist or psychologist
can deliver an opinion on
how can best to effect the
desired
96
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
changes.
But the decision for treatment as a
function of abnormality must be
based on someone's
value
system-it
does not reside in
psychiatry or psychology.
Where
Does This Leave
Us?
As
the previous discussion points
out, all definitions of
abnormal behavior have their
strengths and
weaknesses.
These definitions can
readily incorporate certain
examples of abnormal behavior,
but
exceptions
that do not fit these
definitions are easy to
provide. For example, all of
us can think of an
"abnormal
behavior" that would not be
classified as such if we adopted the
subjective distress
criterion
(for
example, spending sprees in mania), and
we can think of a behavior
that might be
classified
incorrectly
as abnormal if we adopted the violation of
norms definition.
It
is also important to note
that abnormal behavior does
not necessarily indicate mental
illness. Rather,
the
term mental illness refers to a large
class of frequently observed syndromes
that are comprised of
certain
abnormal behaviors or features. These
abnormal behaviors/features tend to
co-vary or occur
together
such that they often
are present in the same
individual. For example,
major depression is a
widely
recognized mental illness whose
features (such as depressed
mood, sleep disturbance,
appetite
disturbance,
and suicidal ideation) tend to co-occur
in the same individual. However, an
individual who
manifested
only one or two of these
features of major depression
would not receive this diagnosis
and
might
not be considered mentally ill.
One can manifest a wide
variety of abnormal behaviors
(as judged
by
any definition), and yet
not receive a mental disorder
diagnosis.
MENTAL
ILLNESS
Like
abnormal behavior, the term
mental illness or mental
disorder is difficult to define.
For any
definition,
exceptions come to mind. Nevertheless, it
seems important to actually
define mental illness
rather
than to assume that we all
share the same implicit
idea of what mental illness
is.
The
fourth edition of the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
(American Psychiatric
Association,
1994), known as DSM-IV the
official diagnostic system
for mental disorders in the
United
States,
states that a mental
disorder is conceptualized as:
"a
clinically significant behavioral or
psychological syndrome or pattern that
occurs in an individual
and
that is associated with
present distress (e.g., a painful
symptom) or disability (i.e.,
impairment in
one
or more important areas of functioning)
or with a significantly increased
risk of suffering, death,
pain,
disability, or an important loss of
freedom".
In
addition, this syndrome or pattern
must not be merely an expectable and
culturally sanctioned
response
to a particular event, for
example, the death of a loved one.
Whatever its original
causes, it
must
currently be considered a manifestation of a
behavioral, psychological, or biological
dysfunction in
the
individual. Neither deviant
behavior (e.g., religious, political, or
sexual) nor conflicts that
are
primarily
between the individual and society
are mental disorders unless the deviance
or conflict is a
symptom
of the dysfunction in the individual as
described above.
CONCLUSION
Several
aspects of this definition
are important to note: (1)
The syndrome (cluster of abnormal
behaviors)
must be associated with
distress, disability, or increased
risk of problems; (2) a
mental
disorder
is considered to represent a dysfunction
within an individual; and (3)
not all deviant
behaviors
or
conflicts with society are
signs of mental
disorder.
The
astute reader has probably
noticed that the DSM-IV
definition of mental disorder
incorporates the
three
definitions of abnormal behavior
presented earlier. On the one hand, the
DSM-IV definition is
more
comprehensive than any one of the three
individual definitions of abnormal
behavior presented
earlier.
On the other hand, the DSM-IV definition
is more restrictive because it focuses on
syndromes,
97
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
or
clusters of abnormal behaviors,
that are associated with
distress, disability, or an increased
risk for
problems.
98
Table of Contents:
|
|||||