|
|||||
Clinical
Psychology (PSY401)
VU
LESSON
11
THE
RESEARCH PROCESS
The
process of research in clinical
psychology, like research in
other areas of psychology
and in other
sciences,
involves the unfolding of a story.
The story begins with a
question that, when framed
properly,
can
be answered with an acceptable
degree of certainty. Pursuing the answers
to questions involves six
broad
steps; generation of hypotheses,
selection of measures of key variables,
selection of a research
design,
selection of a sample, hypothesis testing, and
interpretation and dissemination of
results.
Throughout
this process, it is imperative
that researchers follow clear
guidelines and standards
for
ethical
treatment of participants (human or animal) in
research.
1.
GENERATING HYPOTHESES
Any
piece of research begins with a question
that needs to be answered.
Why do more women than
men
experience
clinical depression? What is the
role of human genetics in the development
of infantile
autism?
How stable are certain
personality characteristics, such as
extroversion or sociability, across
the
life
span? What role does
poverty play in the development of
psychopathology? Is psychotherapy
effective
in the treatment of eating disorders such
as Bulimia Nervosa and
Anorexia Nervosa? These
questions
are among the thousands that have
been posed and examined
through research by
clinical
psychologists.
In
order to serve as the focus of scientific
research, a question needs to be
refined into a hypothesis. A
hypothesis
goes further than a question
in that it reflects the researcher's best
educated idea about
the
expected
answer to a question. Furthermore, a
hypothesis can be tested to determine if
the null
hypothesis
(i.e., that there is no difference or no
relationship between the variables being
studied) can be
rejected
with some degree of
certainty or statistical probability.
Some types of descriptive research
(e.g.,
epidemiological
research to determine the prevalence of
different forms of psychopathology)
may not
be
framed in terms of hypotheses.
Instead, descriptive research
attempts to provide information
that
defines
the extent or parameters of a particular
behavior, personality characteristic, or
psychological
disorder.
Research
that is aimed to predict, explain, or
change human behavior, however, is
best represented as a
specific
hypothesis that can be tested.
The statistical methods
employed by psychologists allow
researchers
to determine the probability that the
null hypothesis is false and that a
hypothesized
relationship
among different variables did
not occur simply by
chance.
Research
hypotheses can emerge from
at least three sources: careful
observations of a clinical case
or
cases,
a theory
concerned with human behavior or
psychopathology, and the
results of
previous
research.
A skilled clinical researcher is
first and foremost an astute observer of human
behavior. One
of
the richest and most relevant sources of
observations is the interactions of clinical
psychologist with
their
clients or patients. For example, a
clinical psychologist who
treats aggressive and
noncompliant
children
may observe that these
children frequently come
from families in which there is a
high degree
of
conflict and anger between the parents.
This observation raises the
possibility that conflict
and
discord
in marital relationships can
contribute to childhood aggression and
noncompliance. Equally
plausible,
however, is the hypothesis that
aggressive and disobedient behavior by a
child can contribute
to
tension and arguments between parents. In
this way, observations of individual
cases raise questions
whose
answers require other
methods and additional
information.
A
theory can serve as an
important source of hypotheses
about a variety of clinical problems. A
theory
serves
as a road map for a clinical
psychologist, providing a sense of
what to expect and why it
should
be
expected. Nevertheless, the central propositions of
any theory cannot be left as
mere abstractions. A
strong
theory must lend itself to
empirical evaluation and testing.
For example, operant behavior
theory
predicts
that any behavior that is
followed by positive consequences
should increase in frequency.
Thus,
a
researcher can do more than
asking a question about the effects of
positive consequences on
behavior.
80
Table of Contents:
|
|||||