|
|||||
Theories
of Communication MCM 511
VU
LESSON
41
FRAMING
AND FRAME ANALYSIS
While
critical cultural researchers were
developing reception analysis during the
1980s, a new approach
to
audience research was taking
shape in the US. This approach had
its roots in theories, Symbolic
interaction
and social construction of reality. Both
these theories argue that the
expectations we form
about
other people, our social
world, and ourselves are one of the basic
elements in social life.
All
these concepts emphasize
that:
1.
Our expectations are based on
previous experience of some kind,
whether derived from a
media
message
or direct personal experience- that is , we aren't
born with them
2.
Can be quite resistant to
change even when they are
contradicted by readily available
factual
information
3.
Are often associated with
and can arouse strong emotions such as
hate, fear, or love
4.
May be free of our conscious
control over them, especially
when strong emotions are aroused
that
interfere
with our ability to make
sense of new information
available in the situation.
Developing
and using expectations is a normal and
routine part of everyday
life. As human beings we
have
cognitive skills that allow
us to continually scan our
environment, make sense of
it, and then act on
these
interpretations. Our inability to
adequately understand these skills in no
way prevents them from
operating,
but it does impede our
ability to make sense of our
own sense making.
Sociologist Erving
Goffman
in 1974 introduced a theory of frame
analysis to provide a systematic account
of how we use
expectations
to make sense of every day
life situations and the people in them.
He argued that we
constantly
and often radically change
the way we define or typify
situations, actions, and other people
as
we
move through time and
space.
In
other words, our experience of the world
is constantly shifting from one
world to another without
noticing
that a boundary has been
crossed. According to him we
don't operate with a limited or
fixed set
of
experience daily existence as having
order and meaning. According to
Goffman we are always
monitoring
the social environment for social
cue that signal when we
are to make a change. He
used the
term
frame to refer to a specific
set of expectations that are
used to make sense of a social
situation at a
given
point of time.
Frames
are like the typication
schemes described by BErger and
Lukman but they differ in
certain
important
respects. When we move from
one set of frames to another, we
downshift
or upshift. We
reframe
situations so that we experience them as
being more or less
serious.
For
example if you were pretending to
fight with a friend but one
of you got hurt and the
fight turned
serious.
You both downshifted. Suddenly,
you no longer pulled punches
but tried to make them
inflict
as
much pain as possible, suddenly,
you no longer pulled punches
but tried to make them inflict as
much
pain
as possible.
Many
of the fighting skills learned during
play were used but with a
different frame-, you were
trying to
hurt
your friend. Perhaps as you
both tired, one of you told
a joke and cued the other
that you wanted to
up
shift and go back to a more playful
frame.
According
to Goffman, daily life
involves countless shifts in
frames and these shifts
are negotiated by
using
social cues. Some cues are
conventional and universal, others
are very subtle and used by
small
groups.
E.g. during the course of
conversation, many up shift
sand downshifts can
occur.
So
where do media come into this
theory? In gender advertisements in 1979 he
presented an insightful
argument
concerning the influence that
advertising could have on our
perception of members of
the
opposite
sex. He argued that advertising
that uses the sex appeal o
women to attract the attention of
men
inadvertently
teaches us social cues that
could have serious consequences. He
showed how women
are
presented
a less serious and more playful
than men in numerous advertisements.
Women smile, wear
126
Theories
of Communication MCM 511
VU
colorful
clothing and in various ways signal
deference and willingness to take
direction from men.
But
could
these representations of women be
teaching social cues that have unexpected
consequences?
We
might be learning more than
product definitions from
these ads . We could be
learning a vast array
of
social cues, some blatant
but others quite subtle.
Once learned, these cues
could be used in daily
life
to
make sense of members of the
opposite sex and to impose frames on
them, their action, and the
situations
in which we encounter them.
Goffman's
theory provides an intriguing
way of assessing how media
can elaborate and reinforce
a
dominant
public culture, advertisers did
not create sex-role
stereotypes, but Goffman
argues that they
have
homogenized how women are
publicly depicted.
Ads
both teach and reinforce
cues. The specific message
that each of us gets form
the ads will be very
different,
but their long-term
consequence may be similar-
dominant myths about women are
retold and
reinforced.
Information
Processing Theory
For
more than two decades,
cognitive psychologists have been
developing an innovative perspective
on
the
way that individuals
routinely cope with sensory
information: information-processing
theory. The
theory
is actually a large set of
quite diverse and disparate ideas
about coping mechanism and
strategies
and
provides yet another way to
study media audience activity.
Researchers work to understand
how
people
take in, process and
store various information
that are provide by
media.
Closely
related to systems theory
information processing theory
uses mechanistic analogies to
describe
and
interpret how each of us
takes in an makes sense of the
flood of information that we receive
from
our
senses every moment of each
day.
This
theory describes individuals as
complex computers with
certain built in information
handling
capacities
and strategies. Each day we
are exposed to vast
quantities of sensory information; we
filter
this
information so that only a
small fraction of it ever
reaches our conscious mind;
then only a tiny
fraction
of this information is singled
out for attention and processing; and
then we finally store a
tiny
fraction
of this in long term
memory.
According
to some cognitive theorists, we are
not so much information handlers as
information
avoiders-
we have developed sophisticated mechanisms
for screening out irrelevant or
useless
information.
Thus, very little of what
goes on around us ever
reaches our consciousness, and
most of
this
is soon forgotten.
Examples
while reading a book if you
are good in focusing
attention on reading, then
your are routinely
screening
out most of these external
and internal stimuli in favor of the
printed words on the page.
But
just
how many of these words are
you actually noticing? How
many will you remembering ten
minutes,
or
ten hours, or ten days.
Example
viewing television is actually a
rather complex task that
uses very different
information
processing
skills than does reading a
textbook; you are exposed to
rapidly changing images and
sounds.
You
must sort these out and pay
attention to those that will
be most useful to you in
achieving whatever
purpose
you have for your
viewing.
But
if this task is so complex,
why does television seem to
such an easy medium to
use?
Because
the task of routinely making
sense of television appears to be
similar to the task of
routinely
making
sense of everyday experience, and making
sense of that experience is easy.
Information
processing theory offers fresh
insight into our routine
handling of information. It
challenges
some
basic assumptions about the
way we take in and use
sensory data. For example,
we assume that we
would
be better off if we could take in more
information and remember it better.
However, more isn't
always
better in the case of information.
Some people actually experience
severe problems because
they
127
Theories
of Communication MCM 511
VU
have
trouble routinely screening out
irrelevant environmental stimuli.
Such people are overly
sensitive
to
meaningless cues such as
background noise of light
shifts.
Another
useful insight from
information processing theory is
recognition of the limitations of
consciousness
awareness. Our culture
places high value on
conscious thought processes, and we
tend to
be
skeptical or suspicious of the utility of
mental processes that are
only indirectly or not at
all subject to
conscious
control.
We
associate consciousness with
rationality - the ability to make wise
decisions based on careful
evaluation
of all available, relevant
information. We associate unconscious
mental processes with
things
like
uncontrolled emotion, wild
intuition, or even mental illness. We
sometimes devalue the
achievements
of athletes because their greatest
acts are typically performed
without conscious
thought.
No
wonder we are reluctant to
acknowledge our great dependency on
unconscious mental
processes.
According
to information processing theory, we
can never be conscious of more
than a very small
fraction
of the information present in our
environment , as we absorb large
quantities of information ,
we
are consciously aware of small
fraction. We have to depend on routinized
processing of information
and
must normally limit
conscious efforts to only
those instances when
intervention is crucial.
Information
Processing Model
According
to information processing theory,
what we need is an ability to
routinely scan our
environment,
taking in, identifying and
routinely structuring the most
useful stimuli and screening
out
irrelevant
stimuli.
Then
we must be able to process the structured
stimuli that we take in,
hold these structures in
memory
long
enough so that we can sort
out the most useful ones,
put the useful ones into the
right categories,
and
then store them in long-term
memory.
Described
in this way the process
seems simple, but cognitive
psychologists are finding that the
process
is
quite complex with many
different information screening skills
and various processing
stages.
Processing
television news
Information
processing theory has been
used most extensively in
mass communication research to
guide
and
interpret research about how
people decode and learn from
television news broadcasts.
Numerous
studies
have been conducted and useful reviews of
this literature are now
available. Remarkably
similar
findings
have been gained from very
different types of research, including
mass audience surveys and
small-scale
laboratory experiments.
Though
most of us view television as an
easy medium to understand and one that
can make us
eyewitnesses
to important events, television is
actually a difficult medium to
use. Frequently,
information
is presented on television in ways that
inhibit rather than facilitate
learning. Part of the
problem
rests with audience members.
Most of us view television
primarily as an entertainment
medium.
We
have developed many disinformation
processing skills and strategies
for watching television
that
serve
us well in making sense of
entertainment content but that
interfere with effective
interpretation
and
recall of new. We approach the news
passively and rely on routine
activation of schemas (more
or
less
highly structured sets of categories or
patterns; sets of interrelated conceptual
categories).
We
rarely engage in deep,
reflective processing of news content, so
most of it is quickly forgotten.
Even
when
we do make a more conscious effort to
learn from news, we often
lack the schemas necessary
to
make
in-depth interpretations of content or to
store these interpretations in
long-term memory.
Similarly,
news broadcasters also bear
part of the blame. The average
newscast is often so hard to make
sense
of that it can be said to be
biased against understanding. The
typical broadcast contains too
many
stories,
each of which tries to
condense too much
information into little
time.
128
Theories
of Communication MCM 511
VU
Stories
are individually packaged
segments that are typically
composed of complex combinations
of
visuals
and verbal content-all too
often the visual information is so
powerful that it overwhelms
the
verbal.
Viewers are left with
striking mental images but
little contextual information.
Often pictures are
used
that totally irrelevant to
stories.
Likewise,
stories with complex structure and
terminology or powerful but
irrelevant visual images
were
poorly
understood. Human -interest stories
with simple but dramatic
storylines were well understood.
Summing
up
The
theories suggest that our
use of media is actually much more
complicated than we might
like to
assume.
Our use of media is an infinitely
complex process.
How
does someone who believes in
the concept of an active audience but
who is also working
to
understand
mass communication do so using
contemporary audience theories?
Elihu
Katz andJay Blumler two of the
creators of the original 1974volume,
The Uses of Mass
Communication
gave the following advices:
1.
Rejection of audience imperialism. Our
stress on audience activity should
not be equated with a
serene
faith in the full or easy
realization of audience autonomy.
2.
Social roles constrain audience needs ,
opportunities and choices...the individual is
part of a
social
structure and his or her choices
are less free and
less random than a vulgar
gratifications
would
presume.
3.
Texts are also to some
extent constraining. In our
zeal to deny a one to one
relationship between
media
content and audience motivation, we have sometimes
appeared to slip into the
less
warranted
claim that almost any type
of content may serve any
type of function.
4.
Their fourth assertion is
that these three propositions
inject into the uses and
gratifications
paradigm
an essential element of realism, - without
reducing ...our normative
commitment to
the
would be active audience member and to
the provision of media materials designed to
enable
him or her to realize his or her
purposes.
129
Table of Contents:
|
|||||