|
|||||
![]() Research
Methods STA630
VU
Lesson
34
EXPERIMENTAL
RESEARCH (Cont.)
Steps
in Conducting an Experiment
Following
the basic steps of the research
process, experimenters decide on a topic,
narrow it into a
testable
research problem or question,
and then develop a hypothesis
with variables. Once a
researcher
has
the hypothesis, the steps of experimental
research are clear. Broadly
there are about 12 steps
in
conducting
an experiment, which are as
below:
1.
Begin
with a straightforward hypothesis that is
appropriate for experimental
research.
2.
Decide
on an experimental design that will
test the hypothesis within practical
limitations. The
researcher
decides the number of groups to use, how
and when to create treatment
conditions,
the
number of times to measure the dependent variable, and
what the groups of subjects
will
experience
from beginning till
end.
3.
Decide
how to introduce the treatment or create
a situation that induces the
independent
variable.
4.
Develop
a valid and reliable measure of the
dependent variable.
5.
Set
up an experimental setting and conduct a
pilot test of the treatment and dependent
variable
measures.
6.
Locate
appropriate subjects or
cases.
7.
Randomly
assign subjects to groups (if random
assignment is used in the chosen
research
design)
and give careful
instructions.
8.
Gather
data for the pretest measure
of the dependent variable for all groups
(if pretest is used
in
thee
chosen design).
9.
Introduce
the treatment to the experimental group
only (or to the relevant groups if there
are
multiple
experimental groups) and monitor all
groups.
10.
Gather
data for posttest measure of
the dependent variable.
11.
Debrief
the subjects by informing them of the
true purpose and reasons for
the experiment. Ask
subjects
what they thought was
occurring. Debriefing is crucial
when subjects have
been
deceived
about some aspect of the
treatment.
12.
Examine
data collected and make
comparisons between different groups. Where
appropriate,
use
statistics and graphs to determine
whether or not the hypothesis is
supported.
Types
of Designs
Researchers
combine parts of experiment (e.g.
pretests, control groups, etc.) together
into an
experimental
design. For example some
designs lack pretests, some
do not have control groups, and
others
have many experimental groups. Certain
widely used standard designs
have names.
Classical
Experimental Design: All
designs are variations of the
classical experimental design,
which
has
random assignment of subjects, a pretest
and a posttest, an experimental group,
and a control group.
Quasi-Experimental
Designs:
One-shot
Case Study Design:
Also
called the one-group posttest-only
design, the one-shot case study
design
has only one group, a
treatment, and a posttest. Because it is
only one group, there is no random
assignment.
For example, a researcher
shows a group of students a
horror film, then measures
their
attitude
with a questionnaire. A weakness of
this design is that it is difficult to
say for sure that
the
treatment
caused the dependent variable. If
subjects were the same before and
after the treatment, the
researcher
would not know
it.
One
Group Pretest-posttest
Design: This
design has one group, a
pretest, a treatment, and a posttest.
It
lacks
a control group and random assignment.
Continuing with the previous
example, the researcher
gives
a group of students an attitude
questionnaire to complete, shows a horror
film, then has them
117
![]() Research
Methods STA630
VU
complete
the same questionnaire second
time. This is an improvement
over the one-shot case
study
because
the researcher measures the dependent
variable both before and
after the treatment. But it
lacks
the
control group for comparison.
The researcher cannot know
whether something other than
the
treatment
occurred between the pretest and the posttest to
cause the outcome.
Two
Groups Posttest-only Design:
It
has two groups, a random
assignment of subjects, a posttest,
and
a
treatment. It has all parts of the
classical design except a pretest.
Continuing with our
previous
example,
the researcher forms two groups
through randomization process. He
shows group a horror
film
to
one group i.e. the experimental
group. The other group is
not shown any film. Both
groups then
complete
the questionnaire. The random assignment
reduces the chance that the groups
differed before
the
treatment, but without a pretest, a
researcher cannot be as certain that the
groups began the same on
the
dependent variable.
True
Experimental Designs
Experimental
designs, which have at least
two groups, a random assignment of
subjects to experimental
and
control groups, only experimental
group is exposed to treatment, both
groups record information
before
and after the treatment, are known as
ex-post facto experimental
designs.
Pretest
and Posttest Experimental and
Control Group Design:
Two
groups, one control group and the
other
experimental group, are
formed randomly. Both the groups
are exposed to pretest and
posttest.
The
experimental group is exposed to
treatment while the control group is
not. Measuring the
difference
between the differences in the post- and pretests of
the two groups would give the net
effects
of
the treatment.
Experimental
Group: Pretest (O1)
X
Posttest
(O2)
Control
Group: Pretest (O3)
-
Posttest
(O4)
Randomization
used for setting up the
group.
[(O2
O1) (O4 O3)] =
Treatment effect (could be
anywhere between 0 to -1 or +1).
Solomon's
Four Group Design:
To
gain more confidence in internal
validity in experimental
designs,
it
is advisable to set up two
experimental groups and two control
groups. One experimental group
and
one
control group can be given
the both pretest and the posttest.
The other two groups will be
given
only
the posttest. Here the effects of treatment
can be calculated in several different
ways as shown in
figure
1:
Figure
1: Solomon's four group design
Group
Pretest
Treatment
Posttest
1.
Experimental
O1
X
O2
2.
Control
O3
-
O4
3.
Experimental
-
X
O5
4.
Control
-
-
O6
(O2
O1) = E
(O4
- O3) = E
(O5
O6) = E
(O5
- O3) = E
[(O2
O1) (O4 O3)] =
E
E
= Effect
118
![]() Research
Methods STA630
VU
If
all Es are similar, the
cause and effect
relationship is highly
valid.
Interaction
Effect
The
effect of two variables
together is likely to be greater than the
individual effect of each
put together.
The
idea of an interaction effect is
familiar, especially in the area of
medicine or illness. As an
example,
imagine
that for a given population
of 100 persons, all of the
same age and sex, it was
found that if all
100
smoked cigarettes the effect would be a
lung cancer rate of 20 percent.
Assume that for an
identical
group
of 100 persons who did
not smoke but lived in a
smoggy environment, 10 percent would get
lung
cancer.
Now consider a third identical
group of 100 persons all of
whom smoke and also live in
a
smoggy
environment. The additive
effect of both smoking and
smog would be 20 percent plus
10
percent,
or a total of 30 percent (30 people)
having cancer. However,
imagine that an actual
medical
survey
of the population shows a cancer rate of
37 percent among persons experiencing
both smoking
and
smog. This extra 7 percent
can be computed residually
as:
Interaction
Effect = Total effect (smoking
effect + smog effect) = 37
percent
=
37 percent - (20 percent + 10 percent)
=
37 percent - 30 percent
=
7 percent
In
experiments we have the pretests and
posttests, in which case we
use the same instrument
for
measuring
the dependent variable, for example
racial prejudice as an effect of a
movie. In pretest is a
questionnaire
in which items forming the
prejudice scale are
dispersed at random among other items
so
that
the subject does not know
that his or her level of
racial prejudice is being
measured. Nevertheless,
the
measurement of this variable
(prejudice) itself, by presenting questions
about race relations
may
stimulate
the subject's thinking and actually
cause a change in his or her
level of racial prejudice.
Any
pretest
effect that occurs will be
visible as part of extraneous change
(change caused by the
test
stimulus)
in the control group, as the pretest is
also presented to the control
group. Any change between
the
pretest and posttest for measuring the
dependent variable in the control group
may be attributed to
the
sensitization of the subjects with the
instrument. In the experimental group of
course a movie (an X
variable)
was shown due to which we expect a change
in the racial prejudice of the subjects.
But that is
not
all. The subjects in the
experimental group were also
exposed to the instrument for measuring
the
racial
prejudice, hence they were
also sensitized. Their posttest
results include the combined
effect of
exposure
to a movie and that of sensitization to
the instrument. In other words the racial
prejudice of
the
subjects in the experimental group
exhibits the interaction effect of the
treatment plus that of
sensitization
of the instrument.
In
order to calculate the interaction effect
in the experiment we shall have two
experimental groups and
one
control group created by
using the randomization process. It
may look like
this:
Experimental
group 1: Pretest (O1)
X
Posttest
(O2)
Control
group:
Pretest
(O3)
-
Posttest
(O4)
Why
O4 be different from O3? The
difference may be due to
sensitization. So let us figure it
out. Let us
take
another experimental group and we do
not pretest i.e. no
sensitization with the
instrument.
Experimental
group 2: No pretest
X
Posttest
(O5)
Let
us work out the results:
(O2-
O1) = D
(O4-
O3) = D/
(O5
O3)= D// (Since all groups
are identical, so we can use
the pretest of any of the
Other
two groups)
Interaction
effect = D [D/ + D//].
Substituting it with our
example of lung
cancer
37
- [10 + 20] = 37 30 = 7
There
are many other experimental
designs like the randomized block design,
Latin square design,
natural
group design, and factorial
design.
119
Table of Contents:
|
|||||