ZeePedia

EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH:The Language of Experiments

<< THE PARTS OF THE TABLE:Reading a percentage Table
EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH (Cont.):True Experimental Designs >>
img
Research Methods ­STA630
VU
Lesson 33
EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH
Experimental research builds on the principles of positivist approach more directly than do the other
research techniques. Researchers in the natural sciences (e.g. chemistry and physics), related applied
fields (e.g. engineering, agriculture, and medicines) and the social sciences conduct experiments. The
logic that guides an experiment on plant growth in biology or testing a metal in engineering is applied in
experiments on human social behavior. Although it is most widely used in psychology, the experiment
is found in education, criminal justice, journalism, marketing, nursing, political science, social work,
and sociology.
The purpose of experimental research is to allow the researcher to control the research situation so that
causal relationships among variables may be evaluated. The experimenter, therefore, manipulates a
single variable in an investigation and holds constant all other, extraneous variables. (Events may be
controlled in an experiment in a way that is not possible in a survey.) The goal of the experimental
design is the confidence that it gives the researcher that his experimental treatment is the cause of the
effect he measures.
Experiment is a research design in which conditions are controlled so that one or more variables can be
manipulated in order to test a hypothesis. Experimentation is a research design that allows evaluation of
causal relationship among variables.
Experiments differ from other research methods in terms of degree of control over the research
situation. In a typical experiment one variable (the independent variable) is manipulated and its effect
on another variable (the dependent variable) is measured, while all other variables that may confound
such relationship are eliminated or controlled. The experimenter either creates an artificial situation or
deliberately manipulates a situation.
Once the experimenter manipulates the independent variable, changes in the dependent variable are
measured. The essence of a behavioral experiment is to do something to an individual and observe his
or her reaction under conditions where this reaction can be measured against a known baseline.
To establish that variable X cause's variable Y, all three of the following conditions should be met:
1. Both X and Y should co-vary (i.e. when one goes up, the other should also simultaneously go
up (or go down).
2. X (the presumed causal factor) should precede Y. In other words, there must be a time sequence
in which the two occur.
3. No other factor should possibly cause the change in the dependent variable Y.
It may thus be seen that to establish causal relationships between two variables in an organizational
setting, several variables that might co-vary with the dependent variable have to be controlled. This
would then allow us to say that variable X and variable X alone causes the dependent variable Y.
Useful as it is to know the cause-and-effect relationships, establishing them is not so easy, because
several other variables that co-vary with the dependent variable have to be controlled. It is not always
possible to control all the co-variates while manipulating the causal factor (the independent variable that
is causing the dependent variable) in organizational settings, where events flow or occur naturally and
normally. It is, however, possible to first isolate thee effects of a variable in a tightly controlled
artificial setting (the lab setting), and after testing and establishing the cause-and-effect relationship
under these tightly controlled conditions, see how generalizable such relationships are to the field
setting.
The Language of Experiments
Experimental research has its own language or set of terms and concepts. One important term frequently
used is subjects or test units. In experimental research, the cases or people used in research projects and
on whom variables are measured are called thee subjects or test units. In other words these are those
112
img
Research Methods ­STA630
VU
entities whose responses to the experimental treatment are measured or observed.  Individuals,
organizational units, sales territories, or other entities may be the test units. Similar terminology is used
on different component parts of the experiments.
Parts of Experiments: We can divide the experiments into seven parts and for each part there is a term.
Not all experiments have all these parts, and some have all seven parts plus others. The following seven
usually make up a true experiment.
1. Treatment or independent variable.
2. Dependent variable.
3. Pretest.
4. Posttest.
5. Experimental group.
6. Control group.
7. Assignment of subjects.
Treatment or independent variable:  The experimenter has some degree of control over thee
independent variable. The variable is independent because its value can be manipulated by the
experimenter to whatever he or she wishes it to be. In experimental design the variable that can be
manipulated to be whatever the experiment wishes. Its value may be changed or altered independently
of any other variable.
In most experiments, a researcher creates a situation or enters into an ongoing situation, then modifies it.
The treatment (or the stimulus or manipulation) is what the researcher modifies. The term comes from
medicine, in which a physician administers a treatment to patients; the physician intervenes in a physical
or psychological condition to change it.  It is the independent variable or the combination of
independent variables.
In experiments, for example, the researcher creates a condition or situation. Look at "the degree of fear
or anxiety"; the levels are high-fear or low-fear situation. Instead of asking the subjects, as we do in
surveys, whether they are fearful, experimenter puts the subjects into either in a high-fear or low-fear
situation. They measure the independent variable by manipulating conditions so that some subjects feel
a lot of fear and others feel little.
Researchers go to great lengths to create treatments. They want the treatment to have an impact and
produce specific reactions, feelings, or behaviors.
It also possible the researchers look at the alternative manipulations of the independent variable being
investigated.  In business research, the independent variable is often categorical or classificatory
variable, representing some classifiable or qualitative aspects of management strategy. To determine
the effects of training, for example, the experimental treatment that represents the independent variable
is the training program itself.
Dependent Variable: The criterion or standard by which thee results are judged. It is assumed that the
changes in the dependent variable are consequence of changes in the independent variable. For example,
measures of turnover, absenteeism, or morale might be alternative choices for the dependent variable,
depending on the purpose of the training.
The outcomes in the experimental research are the physical conditions, social behaviors, attitudes,
feelings, or beliefs of subjects that change, in response to treatment. Dependent variables can be
measured by paper-and-pencil indicators, observations, interviews, or physiological responses (e.g.
heartbeat, or sweating palms).
Selection of dependent variable is crucial decision in the design of an experiment.
113
img
Research Methods ­STA630
VU
Pretests and Posttests: Frequently a researcher measures thee dependent variable more than once during
an experiment. The pretest is the measurement of the dependent variable prior to the introduction of the
treatment. The posttest is the measurement of the dependent variable after thee treatment has been
introduced into the experimental situation.
Experimental and Control Groups: Experimental researchers often divide subjects into two or more
groups for purposes of compassion. A simple experiment has only two groups, only one of which
receives the treatment. The experimental group is the group that receives the treatment or in which the
treatment is present.
The group that does not receive the treatment is called the "control group." When the independent
variable takes on many different values, more than one experimental group is used.
In the simplest type of experiment, only two values of the independent variable are manipulated. For
example, consider measuring the influence of a change in work situation, such as playing music over an
intercom during working hours, on employee productivity. In the experimental condition (the treatment
administered to the experimental group), music is played during working hours. In the control
condition (the treatment administered to the control group), the work situation remains the same,
without change. By holding conditions constant in the control group, the researcher controls for
potential sources of error in the experiment. Productivity, (the dependent variable) in the two groups is
compared at the end of the experiment to determine whether playing the music (the independent
variable) has any effect.
Several experimental treatment levels can also be used. The music/productivity experiment, with one
experimental and one control group, may not tell the researcher everything he or she wishes to know
about the music/productivity relationship. If the researcher wished to understand the functional nature
of the relationship between music and productivity at several treatment levels, additional experimental
groups with music played for only 2 hours, only for 4 hours, and only for 6 hours might be studied. This
type of design would allow the experimenter to get a better idea about the impact of music on
productivity.
Assignment of Subjects/Test Units: Social researchers frequently want to compare. When making
comparisons, the researchers want to compare the cases that do not differ with regard to variables that
offer alternative explanations. Therefore the groups should be similar in characteristics in such a way
that the change in the dependent variable is presumably the outcome of the manipulation of the
independent variable, having no alternative explanations.
Random assignment (Randomization) is a method for assigning the cases (e.g. individuals,
organizations) to groups for the purpose of making comparisons. It is a way to divide or sort a
collection of cases into two or more groups in order to increase one's confidence that the groups do not
differ in a systematic way. It is a mechanical method; the assignment is automatic, and thee researcher
cannot make assignments on thee basis of personal preference or the features of specific cases.
Random assignment is random in statistical/mathematical sense, not in everyday sense. In everyday
speech, random means unplanned, haphazard, or accidental, but it has a special meaning in
mathematics. In probability theory, random describes a process in which each case has a known chance
of being selected. Random selection allows the researcher calculate the odds that a specific case will be
sorted into one group or the other. A random process is the one in which all cases have an exactly equal
chance of ending up in one or the other group.
Random assignment or randomization is unbiased because a researcher's desire to confirm a hypothesis
or a research subject's personal interests does not enter into the selection process. It also assures the
researcher that repetitions of an experiment ­ under the controlled conditions ­ will show true effects, if
114
img
Research Methods ­STA630
VU
they exist. Random assignment of subjects allows the researcher to assume that thee groups are
identical with respect to all variables except for experimental treatment.
Random assignment of subjects to the various experimental groups is thee most common technique used
to prevent test units from differing from each other on key variables; it assumes that all the
characteristics of these subjects have been similarly randomized. If the experimenter believes that
certain extraneous variable may affect the dependent variable, he or she may make sure that the subjects
in each group are matched on these characteristics. Matching the subjects on the basis of pertinent
background information is another technique for controlling assignment errors.
Matching presents a problem: What are the relevant characteristics to match on, and can one locate
exact matches? Individual cases differ in thousands of ways, and the researcher cannot know which
might be relevant.
Three Types of Controls
1. Manipulation of the Independent Variable: In order to examine the causal effects of an
independent variable on a dependent variable, certain manipulations need to be tried.
Manipulation simply means control over the stimulus that is we create different levels of the
independent variable to assess the impact on the dependent variable. Let us say we want to test
the effects of lighting on worker production levels among sewing machine operators. To
establish cause and effect relationship, we must measure the production levels of all the
operators over a 15 day period with the usual amount of light they work with ­ say 60 watt
bulbs. We might then want to split the group of 60 operators into three groups of 20 members
each, and while allowing the subgroup to continue to work under the same conditions as before
(60-watt electric light bulbs). We might want to manipulate the intensity of the light for the
other two subgroups, by making one group work with 75 watt and the other with 100 watt light
bulbs. After the different groups have worked with these varying degrees of light exposure
for15 days, each group's total production for these 15 days may be analyzed to see the
difference between the pre-experimental and the post experimental productions among the
groups is directly related to the intensity of the light to which they have been exposed. If our
hypothesis that better lighting increases the production levels is correct, the subgroups that did
not have any change in the lighting (control group), should have no increase in production and
thee other two groups should show increases, with the one having the most light (100 watts)
showing greater increases than those who had the 75 watt lighting.
In this case the independent variable, lighting, has been manipulated by exposing different
groups to different degrees of changes in it. This manipulation of the independent variable is
also known treatment, and the results of the treatment are called treatment effects.
2. Holding Conditions Constant: When we postulate cause-and-effect relationships between two
variables X and Y, it is possible that some other factor, say A, might also influence the
dependent variable Y. In such a case, it will not be possible to determine the extent to which Y
occurred only because of X, since we do not know how much of the total variation of was
caused by the presence of the other factor A. If the true effect of thee X is to be assessed, then
the effect of A has to be controlled. This is also called as controlling the effect of contaminating
factors or confounding factors.
3. Control over the Composition of Groups: If the experimental and control groups have such
characteristics that could contaminate the results then the researcher may have to take note of
such factors, if there are any. The group differences should not confound the effect of X
variable that happens to be under study. The experimental and control groups need to be
balanced. For this purpose the researcher may use random selection of the subjects and
allocating to different groups. Finally the experimental and control groups should also be
selected randomly. Another way to have identical groups is by following the procedure of
115
img
Research Methods ­STA630
VU
matching. One could look at the possible characteristics of the subjects that could contaminate
the effect of X variable, and try to distribute these evenly in all the groups. So pick up one
subject and try to match it with another subject on the specified characteristics (age, gender,
education, marital status) and put one subject in one group and the other in the other group.
After the formation of groups, the researcher may randomly decide about experimental and
control groups.
Random Assignment
Social researchers frequently want to compare. For example, a researcher has two groups of 15 students
and wants to compare the groups on the basis of key differences between them (e.g. a course that one
group completed). Or a researcher has five groups of customers and wants to compare the groups on the
basis of one characteristic (e.g. geographic location). "Compare apples with apples, don't compare
apples with oranges."  It means that a valid comparison depends on comparing things that are
fundamentally alike. Random assignment facilitates comparison in experiments by creating similar
groups.
Random assignment is a method for assignment cases (e.g. individuals, organizations) to groups for the
purpose of making comparisons. It is a way to divide or sort a collection of cases into two or more
groups in order to increase one's confidence that the groups do not differ in a systematic way. It is
mechanical method; the assignment is automatic, and the researcher cannot make assignments on the
basis of personal preference or the features of specific cases.
Random assignment is random in a statistical or mathematical sense, not in an everyday sense. In
everyday speech, random means unplanned, haphazard, or accidental, but it has a specialized meaning
in mathematics. In probability theory, random describes a process in which each case has a known
chance of being selected. Random assignment lets a researcher calculate the odds that a specific case
will be sorted into one group over another.
Random assignment or randomization is unbiased because a researcher's desire to confirm a hypothesis
or a research subject's personal interest does not enter into selection process.
Matching
It implies to match the characteristics (such as age, sex) of the cases in each group. Matching is an
alternative to random assignment, but it is an infrequently used one.
Matching presents a problem: What are the relevant characteristics to match on, and can one locate
exact matches. Individual cases differ in thousands of ways, and the researcher cannot know which
might be relevant.  Therefore, randomization is preferred over matching.  It takes care of the
contaminating factors.
116
Table of Contents:
  1. INTRODUCTION, DEFINITION & VALUE OF RESEARCH
  2. SCIENTIFIC METHOD OF RESEARCH & ITS SPECIAL FEATURES
  3. CLASSIFICATION OF RESEARCH:Goals of Exploratory Research
  4. THEORY AND RESEARCH:Concepts, Propositions, Role of Theory
  5. CONCEPTS:Concepts are an Abstraction of Reality, Sources of Concepts
  6. VARIABLES AND TYPES OF VARIABLES:Moderating Variables
  7. HYPOTHESIS TESTING & CHARACTERISTICS:Correlational hypotheses
  8. REVIEW OF LITERATURE:Where to find the Research Literature
  9. CONDUCTING A SYSTEMATIC LITERATURE REVIEW:Write the Review
  10. THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK:Make an inventory of variables
  11. PROBLEM DEFINITION AND RESEARCH PROPOSAL:Problem Definition
  12. THE RESEARCH PROCESS:Broad Problem Area, Theoretical Framework
  13. ETHICAL ISSUES IN RESEARCH:Ethical Treatment of Participants
  14. ETHICAL ISSUES IN RESEARCH (Cont):Debriefing, Rights to Privacy
  15. MEASUREMENT OF CONCEPTS:Conceptualization
  16. MEASUREMENT OF CONCEPTS (CONTINUED):Operationalization
  17. MEASUREMENT OF CONCEPTS (CONTINUED):Scales and Indexes
  18. CRITERIA FOR GOOD MEASUREMENT:Convergent Validity
  19. RESEARCH DESIGN:Purpose of the Study, Steps in Conducting a Survey
  20. SURVEY RESEARCH:CHOOSING A COMMUNICATION MEDIA
  21. INTERCEPT INTERVIEWS IN MALLS AND OTHER HIGH-TRAFFIC AREAS
  22. SELF ADMINISTERED QUESTIONNAIRES (CONTINUED):Interesting Questions
  23. TOOLS FOR DATA COLLECTION:Guidelines for Questionnaire Design
  24. PILOT TESTING OF THE QUESTIONNAIRE:Discovering errors in the instrument
  25. INTERVIEWING:The Role of the Interviewer, Terminating the Interview
  26. SAMPLE AND SAMPLING TERMINOLOGY:Saves Cost, Labor, and Time
  27. PROBABILITY AND NON-PROBABILITY SAMPLING:Convenience Sampling
  28. TYPES OF PROBABILITY SAMPLING:Systematic Random Sample
  29. DATA ANALYSIS:Information, Editing, Editing for Consistency
  30. DATA TRANSFROMATION:Indexes and Scales, Scoring and Score Index
  31. DATA PRESENTATION:Bivariate Tables, Constructing Percentage Tables
  32. THE PARTS OF THE TABLE:Reading a percentage Table
  33. EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH:The Language of Experiments
  34. EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH (Cont.):True Experimental Designs
  35. EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH (Cont.):Validity in Experiments
  36. NON-REACTIVE RESEARCH:Recording and Documentation
  37. USE OF SECONDARY DATA:Advantages, Disadvantages, Secondary Survey Data
  38. OBSERVATION STUDIES/FIELD RESEARCH:Logic of Field Research
  39. OBSERVATION STUDIES (Contd.):Ethical Dilemmas of Field research
  40. HISTORICAL COMPARATIVE RESEARCH:Similarities to Field Research
  41. HISTORICAL-COMPARATIVE RESEARCH (Contd.):Locating Evidence
  42. FOCUS GROUP DISCUSSION:The Purpose of FGD, Formal Focus Groups
  43. FOCUS GROUP DISCUSSION (Contd.):Uses of Focus Group Discussions
  44. REPORT WRITING:Conclusions and recommendations, Appended Parts
  45. REFERENCING:Book by a single author, Edited book, Doctoral Dissertation