|
|||||
![]() Research
Methods STA630
VU
Lesson
15
MEASUREMENT
OF CONCEPTS
In
everyday usage, measurement
occurs when an established yardstick
verifies the height, weight,
or
another
feature of a physical object.
How well you like a
song, a painting, or the personality of a
friend
is
also measurement. In a dictionary
sense, to measure is to discover the
extent, dimensions, quantity, or
capacity
of something, especially by comparison with a
standard. We measure casually in
daily life, but
in
research the requirements for measurement
are rigorous.
Certain
things lend themselves to
easy measurement through the
use of appropriate instruments, as
for
example,
physiological phenomena pertaining to
human beings such as blood pressure,
pulse rates, and
body
temperature, as well as certain physical
attributes such as height
and weight. But when we get
into
the
realm of people's subjective
feelings, attitudes, ideology, deviance,
and perceptions, the
measurement
of these factors or variables becomes
difficult. Like the natural scientist
who invents
indirect
measures of the "invisible" objects and
forces of the physical world (magnetism
the force that
moves
a metal toward the magnet), the social
researcher devises measures
for difficult- to-observe
aspects
of the social world. For
example, suppose you heard a
principal complain about
teacher morale
in
a school. Teacher morale is an empirical
reality, and we can create
some instrument for
its
measurement.
Measurement
in Quantitative and Qualitative
Research
Both
qualitative and quantitative researchers
use careful, systematic
methods to gather high quality
data.
Yet,
differences in the styles of research and
the types of data mean they approach the
measurement
process
differently. Designing precise ways to
measure variables is a vital
step in planning a study
for
quantitative
researchers. Qualitative researchers
use wider variety of techniques to
measure and create
new
measures while collecting data.
The two approaches to
measurement have three
distinctions.
One
difference between the two styles
involves timing. Quantitative
researchers extensively think
about
variables
and convert them into specific actions
during a planning stage that
occurs before and
separate
from
gathering or analyzing data. Measurement
for qualitative researchers
occurs in the data
collection
process,
and only a little occurs in
a separate, planning stage
prior to data
gathering.
A
second difference involves the
data itself. Quantitative
researchers want to develop techniques
that
can
produce quantitative data (i.e.
data in the form of numbers). Thus, the
researcher moves from
abstract
ideas, or variables, to specific
data collection techniques to precise
numerical information
produced
by the techniques. The numerical
information is an empirical representation of the
abstract
ideas.
Data for qualitative researchers
sometimes is in the form of numbers; more
often it includes
written
or spoken word, actions, sounds, symbols,
physical objects, or visual images.
The qualitative
researcher
does not convert all
observations into a single, common medium
such as numbers. Instead
he
or
she develops many flexible,
ongoing processes to measure
that leaves the data in
various shapes,
sizes,
and forms.
All
researchers combine ideas
and data to analyze the social world. In
both research styles, data
are
empirical
representation of concepts, and measurement is a
process that links data to
concepts. A third
difference
is how the two styles make
such linkages. Quantitative
researchers contemplate and
reflect
on
concepts before they gather data.
They construct measurement techniques
that bridge concepts
and
data.
The measurement techniques define
what the data will be and
are directions for gathering
data.
Qualitative
researchers also reflect on
ideas before data
collection, but they develop
many, if not most,
of
their concepts during data
collection activities. Researchers
start gathering data and
creating ways to
measure
based what they encounter.
As they gather data, they reflect on the
process and develop
new
ideas.
The ideas give them
direction and suggest new ways to
measure.
46
![]() Research
Methods STA630
VU
Here
we shall focus on quantitative
measurement. Here measurement
consists of assigning numbers to
empirical
events in compliance with
set rules. This definition
implies that measurement is a
three-part
process:
1.
Selecting
observable empirical events.
2.
Developing
a set of mapping rules: a
scheme for assigning numbers or symbols
to represent
aspects
of the event being
measured.
3.
Applying
the mapping rule(s) to each
observation of that
event.
Assume
you are studying people
who attend an auto show where all
year's new models are on
display.
You
are interested in learning the male-to
female ratio among attendees. You
observe those who
enter
the
show area. If a person is female,
you record an F; if male, an M. Any other symbols
such as 0 and 1
may
also be used if you know
what group the symbol
identifies.
Researchers
might also want to measure
the desirability of the styling of the
new Espace van.
They
interview
a sample of visitors and
assign, with a different
mapping rule, their opinions
to the following
scale:
What
is your opinion of the styling of the
Espace van?
Very
desirable 5_______4_______3_______2________1
Very undesirable
We
can assign a weight-age
(score) like:
5
if it is very desirable
4
if desirable
3
if neither
2
if undesirable
1
if very undesirable.
All
measurement theorists would call
such opinion rating scale as
a form of measurement.
What
is measured?
Variable
being studied in research
may be classified as objects or as
properties. Objects
include
the
things
of ordinary experience, such as tables,
people, books, and
automobiles. Objects also
include
things
that are not as concrete,
such as genes, attitudes, neutrons, and
peer group pressures.
Properties
are
the characteristics of the objects. A person's
physical properties may be stated in
terms of weight,
height,
and posture. Psychological properties
include attitudes, intelligence,
motivation, perceptions,
etc.
Social properties include leadership
ability, class affiliation, or
status. These and many
other
properties
of an individual can be measured in a
research study.
In
a literal sense, researchers do
not measure either objects or
properties. They measure
indicants of the
properties
or indicants of the properties of the objects.
The properties like age,
years of experience, and
the
number of calls made per week are
easier to indicate and there is expected to be
lot of agreement.
In
contrast, it is not easy to measure
properties like "motivation,"
"ability to stand stress,"
"problem-
solving
ability," and "persuasiveness." Since
each property cannot be measured
directly, one must
infer
its
presence or absence by observing
some indicant or pointer
measurement. When you begin
to make
these
inferences, there is often disagreement
about how to operationalize the
indicants.
The
preceding discussion suggests
two types of variables: one lends
itself to objective and
precise
measurement;
the other is more nebulous and does not
lend itself to accurate
measurement because of
its
subjective
nature. However, despite the lack of
physical measuring devices to measure the
latter type,
there
are ways to tapping the subjective
feelings and perceptions of individuals.
One technique is to
reduce
the abstract notions, or concepts
such as motivation, involvement,
satisfaction, buyer
behavior,
47
![]() Research
Methods STA630
VU
stock
market exuberance, and the like, to observable
behavior and characteristics. In other
words, the
abstract
notions are broken down
into observable characteristic behavior.
Reducing the abstract
concepts
to render them measurable in a tangible
way is called operationalizing
thee concepts.
Parts
of the Measurement Process
When
a researcher measures, he or she
takes a concept, idea, or construct and develops a
measure (i.e. a
technique,
a process, a procedure) by which he or
she can observe the idea
empirically. Quantitative
researchers
primarily follow a deductive
route. To begin with the
abstract idea, follow with
a
measurement
procedure, and end with empirical
data that represent the
ideas. Qualitative
researchers
primarily
follow inductive route. They
begin with empirical data,
follow with abstract ideas,
follow
with
processes relating with
ideas and data, and end with
a mixture of ideas and data.
Researchers
use two processes:
conceptualization and operationalization in
measurement.
a.
Conceptualization
Conceptualization
is the process of taking a construct and
refining it by giving it a conceptual
or
theoretical
definition. A conceptual definition is
definition in abstract, theoretical
terms. It refers to
other
ideas or constructs. There is no
magical way to turn a construct
into a precise conceptual
definition.
It involves thinking carefully,
observing directly, consulting
with others, reading what
others
have
said, and trying possible
definitions.
A
good definition has one
clear, explicit, and specific
meaning. There is no ambiguity or
vagueness in
the
concepts (e.g. street gang, morale,
motivation, social class, consumer
satisfaction). A single
construct
can have several definitions, and people
may disagree over
definitions. Conceptual
definitions
are
linked to theoretical frameworks and to
value positions. For
example, a conflict theorist
may define
social
class as the
power and property a group of
people in society has or lacks. A
structural
functionalist
defines it in terms of individuals
who share a social status,
life-style, or subjective
identification.
Although people disagree
over definitions, the researcher
should always state
explicitly
which
definition he or she is
using.
Before
you can measure, you
need a concept. You also need to
distinguish what you are
interested in
from
other things. The idea
that you first need a
construct or concept of what is to be
measured simply
makes
sense. How can you
observe or measure something
unless you know what
you are looking
for?
For
example, we want to measure
teacher morale. We first
define teacher
morale.
What does the
construct
morale
mean?
As a variable construct, it takes on
different values high versus
low or good
versus
bad morale. Next we create a
measure of this construct. This
could take the form of
survey
questions,
an examination of school records, or observations of
teachers. Also we distinguish
morale
from
other things in the answers to
survey questions, school records, or
observations.
How
can we develop a conceptual definition of
teacher
morale, or at
least a tentative working
definition
to
get started? Look in the everyday
understanding of morale something vague
like "how people
feel
about
things." Also look in the
dictionary, which gives
definitions like "confidence,
spirit, zeal,
cheerfulness,
esprit de corps, and mental
condition towards something." Look
into the review of
literature
and see how other
researchers have defined this concept. In
this effort we collect
various
definitions,
parts of definitions, and related
ideas, whereby we draw the boundaries of
the core idea.
We
find that most of these
definitions say that morale
is a spirit, feeling, or mental
condition toward
something,
or a group feeling. But we
are interested in teacher
morale.
We can ask teachers to
specify
as
what does this construct
mean to them? One strategy is to
make a list of examples of
high or low
teacher
morale. High teacher morale
includes saying positive
things about the school, not
complaining
about
extra-work or enjoying being
with students. Low
morale includes complaining a
lot, not
attending
school events unless required
to, or looking for other
jobs.
48
![]() Research
Methods STA630
VU
Morale
involves a feeling toward something
else; a person has morale
with regard to something. A list
of
various "somethings" toward
which teachers have feelings (e.g.
students, parents, pay, the
school
administration,
other teachers, the profession of
teaching). Are there several kinds of
teacher morale or
all
these "somethings" aspects of
one construct? We have to decide whether
morale means a
single,
general
feeling with different parts
or dimensions, or several distinct
feelings.
What
unit of analysis does our construct
apply to: a group or an
individual? Is morale a characteristic
of
an
individual, of a group, or of
both?
A
researcher must distinguish the construct
of interest from related constructs.
How is our construct of
teacher
morale similar to or different
from related concepts? For
example, does morale
differ
from
mood?
We decide that mood is more individual
and temporary than morale.
Morale is a group
feeling
that
includes positive or negative
feelings about the future as
well as other beliefs and
feelings.
Who
is a teacher? We have to decide.
49
Table of Contents:
|
|||||