/96-19_files/96-1900001im.jpg" width="718"
height="1022" useMap="#Map">
VU
Sport
Psychology (PSY407)
Lesson
19
ANXIETY,
AROUSAL, AND STRESS
RELATIONSHIPS
This
lecture is a continuation of lecture
eighteen. We were looking at anxiety,
arousal, and stress
relationship,
and had divided this topic
into six sections, they
were:
1.
Differentiating
among the terms anxiety, arousal
and stress
2.
The
multidimensional nature of anxiety
3.
Antecedents
of anxiety
4.
Measurement
of anxiety
5.
Time-to-event
nature of precompetitive anxiety
6.
The
relationship between anxiety and
performance
We
have looked at the first
three sections in lecture
eighteen and the remaining will be
discussed in this
lecture.
Measurement
of Anxiety
In
recent years, the preferred method of
measuring trait and state
anxiety has been through the
use of
pencil-and-paper
inventories. Some commonly used inventories
utilized or developed by sport
psychologists
are listed below:
Sport
Competition Anxiety Test
(SCAT)
TRAIT
Unidimensional
Multidimensional
Sport
Anxiety Scale (SAS)
Unidimensional
Competitive
State Anxiety Inventory
(CSAI)
STATE
Competitive
State Anxiety Inventory-2
(CSAI-2)
Multidimensional
While
pencil-and-paper inventories are the most common
measures of anxiety, behavioral
and
psychological
assessments can be very effective. One
category of behavioral measurement is
direct
observation,
where the experimenter looks for objective
signs of arousal in the subject
and records
them.
Such things as nervous fidgeting,
licking the lips, rubbing
palms on pants or shirt, and
change in
respiration
could all be interpreted as behavioral
signs of activation. The list on the
next page shows
overt
behavioral responses that can be
used by the athlete to identify
indicators of distress, or state
anxiety.
Checklist
for Monitoring Distress-Related
Behavioral Responses of the
Athlete:
Clammy
Hands
Tense
Muscles
Diarrhea
Tense
Stomach
Dry
Mouth
Trembling
Legs
Fidgeting
Unsettled
Stomach
Increased
Respiration
Voice
Distortion
Irritability
Jitters
Licking
of Lips
Mental
Confusion
Mental
Fatigue
Nausea
Need
to Urinate
Physical
Fatigue
Rapid
Heart Rate
Scattered
Attention
64
/96-19_files/96-1900002im.jpg" width="695"
height="1066" useMap="#Map">
Sport
Psychology(psy407)
VU
Time-To-Event
Nature of Precompetitive
Anxiety
The
ability to obtain independent measures of
cognitive and somatic state
anxiety has greatly enhanced
our
knowledge
about the athletic situation. One of the factors
that is believed to significantly influence the
quality of
the
athletic experience is the level of state anxiety
during the time leading up to
competition. We have
already
referred
to this as precompetitive anxiety.
Precompetitive
cognitive anxiety starts relatively high
and remains high and
stable as the time-to-event
approaches.
Conversely, somatic anxiety remains
relatively low until approximately
twenty-four hours before the
event,
and then increases rapidly
as the event approaches. Once performance
begins, somatic anxiety
dissipates
rapidly,
whereas cognitive state anxiety
fluctuates throughout the contest as the
probability of success/failure
changes.
The
Relationship between Arousal and Athletic
Performance
It
is now necessary to use the term
arousal as somewhat synonymous
with state anxiety. This is the case
because
researchers
have routinely employed a
test of state anxiety as the primary
means for determining a
subject's
arousal
level. Consequently, most of the reported
research will relate
negative anxiety to sport and
motor
performance.
The
primary focus is to explain the relationship between
arousal and athletic performance: It
can be explained
by
inverted-U theory and drive
theory.
Inverted-U
theory explains why the relationship
between arousal and
performance is curvilinear as opposed
to
linear
in nature. Conversely, drive
theory proposes a linear relationship
between arousal and
performance.
The
Inverted-U Theory
The
inverted U theory has been
around for as long as the arousal/performance
relationship has been studied.
It
simply
states that the relationship between
performance and arousal is curvilinear as
opposed to linear, and
takes
the
form of an inverted-U.
One
of the difficulties encountered in
testing the inverted U theory
with humans is our inability
to precisely
measure
arousal. For example, if in a particular
study researchers fail to
demonstrate that heightened
arousal
causes
a decrement in performance, it is not
particularly damaging to the theory. The
reason for this is that
it
can
always be argued that for
that particular task, arousal
was not high enough. If it
had been higher
performance
would have been declined.
The problem is that from a
human rights standpoint, the amount of
arousal
researchers can induce is
limited. For example, if
arousal is induced through electric
shock, how much
can
the researcher elevate the voltage
without violating the subject's
rights?
Similarly
as can be observed, a high level of
arousal is necessary for the
best performance in gross
motor
activities
such as weight lifting. Conversely, a
lower level of arousal is best
for a fine motor task
such as putting
in
golf. Each sport skill has
its theoretical optimal level of arousal
for best performance.
Regardless of which
type
of skill is being performed, they all
conform to the inverted-U principle.
Specifically, performance is
lowest
when
arousal is very high or very low,
and highest when arousal is
moderate, or optimum.
Evidence
of an inverted-U relationship between athletic
performance and arousal is
documented in the
literature.
Klavora (1978), Sonstroem
and Bernardo (1982), were
able to demonstrate that
basketball
performance
is related to level of arousal, with
best performance occurring at moderate
levels of arousal and
poorest
performance tat high ot low
levels. Similarly Simons, and
Vevera (1987) and Burton
(1988) reported
that
best performance in pistol shooting
and swimming, respectively,
were related to somatic anxiety in
away
consistent
with inverted U predictions.
While
it seems relatively clear that the
nature of the relationship between athletic
performance and arousal
takes
the
form of the inverted U, it is not
clear why this
occurs.
Drive
Theory
The
great contribution of drive
theory is that it helps to explain the
relationships between learning and
arousal,
and
between performance and
arousal. Many young athletes
are just beginning the
process of becoming skilled
performers.
The effect of arousal upon a beginner
may be different from its
effect upon a skilled performer.
The
basic relationship between arousal
and an athlete's performance at
any skill level is given in the
following
formula:
©
Copyright Virtual University of
Pakistan
65
/96-19_files/96-1900003im.jpg" width="695"
height="1066" useMap="#Map">
Sport
Psychology(psy407)
VU
Performance
= Arousal x Skill
Level
The
basic tenets of drive theory
are as follows:
1.
Increased arousal
(drive) will elicit the
dominant response.
2.
The response
associated with the strongest
potential to respond is the dominant
response.
3.
Early in learning or for complex
tasks, the dominant response is the
incorrect response.
4.
Late in learning or for simple
task, the dominant response is the
correct response.
We
can make several practical
applications of these drive theory
tenets. First, heightened levels
of
arousal
should benefit the skilled performer, but
hamper the beginner. The coach
with a relatively
young
team should strive to create an
atmosphere relatively low in anxiety and
arousal. Low levels
of
arousal
should increase the beginner's chances of
a successful performance. In turn, the
experience of
success
should strengthen self confidence.
Skilled athletes, on the other hand,
will benefit from an
increase
in arousal. Similar applications can be
made to the performance of simple
and complex tasks.
For
example, a complex task, such as
throwing a knuckleball in baseball,
will always require a low
level
of
arousal. Conversely, a very simple
task, such as doing as high
number of push-ups, would seem
to
benefit
from arousal. Utilizing
drive theory predictions, the researchers
hypothesized that
increased
arousal
caused by major league baseball
pressure situations would
cause a decrement in batting (
a
complex
task). Four late-game
pressure situations were
compared with no pressure
situations relative to
batting
performance. Results showed a
decrement in batting performance
associated with
increased
arousal,
as predicted by drive theory.
Drive
theory received tremendous
amounts of attention from
researchers between 1943 and
1970.
however,
since then, interest in the theory
has diminished significantly. The
theory was extremely
difficult
to test, and the tests that
were conducted often yielded
conflicting results.
References
Cox,
H. Richard. (2002). Sport Psychology:
Concepts and Applications.
(Fifth Edition). New
York:
McGraw-Hill
Companies
Lavallec.
D., Kremer, J., Moran,
A., & Williams. M. (2004)
Sports Psychology: Contemporary
Themes.
New
York: Palgrave Macmillan
Publishers
©
Copyright Virtual University of
Pakistan
66